Related Questions
- Lab Report , Specifically Results Section, Write Up
- Write Assignment In Advanced Statistics.
- Help With Sociological Research Questions Examples
- Implicit And Explicit Bias That I Have Or Not
- Politics/Policy Issues Essay
- Essay On Policy Issues Pros And Cons Of Each Side And More.... 2500 Word Min.
- Multiple Choice And Short Written Answers
- American Government Democracy
- American Government Democracy
- Take Organizational Behavior Exam
- Social Psychology Comprehensive
- I'm Unsure How To Create An Analysis Plan For My Data
- Psychology Exam 50 Multiple Choice Questions
- Psychology Exam 50 Multiple Choice Questions
- Miterm Exam Organisational Structure. Harvard Reference. B+
Popular Services
- Coursework writing help
- Term paper writing help
- Writing Help
- Paper Writing Help
- Research paper help
- Thesis Help
- Dissertation Help
- Case study writing service
- Capstone Project Writing Help
- Lab report Writing
- Take my online class
- Take my online exam
- Do my test for me
- Do my homework for me
- Do my math homework for me
- Online Assignment Help
- Do my assignment for me
- Essay Writing Help
- Write my college essay
- Write my essay for me
Post your project now for free and watch professional homework help experts outbid each other in just a few minutes.
What Is The Meaning Of Friendhship: Conduct An Inductive Thematic Analysis On The Data. Lab Report.
Asked
Modified
Viewed
35
Select a minimum of 5 pages (max. 10 pages) from each of the transcripts (Alexanders interview and Louises interview)
Conduct an inductive thematic analysis on the data
Possible question - What is the meaning of friendship?
Write up a report based on your analysis (maximum word limit 2500) but I would be happy with even 1000 words
i am also happy to negotiate prices, thank you.
This order does not have tags, yet.
Additional Instructions
Data:
Louise’s Interview
Alexander’s interview
How did you choose your course of study?
How do employers choose among job candidates?
Would you trust any standardized battery of assessments sufficiently to choose your partner based solely on the results?
Task......
1 = very poor to 5 = excellent
How would you rate the job ‘nurse’ ?
Explain your answer to a person sitting next to you
Compare the rating to the verbal explanation
Positivism and the ‘standard view’ of science
‘the external world itself determines absolutely the one and only correct view that can be taken of it, independent of the process or circumstances of the viewing’ (Kirk and Miller, 1986: 14).
objective knowledge;
understanding that is unbiased and impartial;
based on a view from ‘the outside’
Nagel (1986) - the ‘view from nowhere’
Features of the positivist approach
objective knowledge (facts) can be gained from direct experience or observation, and is the only knowledge available to science; invisible or theoretical entities are rejected;
science separates facts from values; it is value-free
purpose of science is to develop universal causal laws by finding empirical regularities where two or more things appear together (a ‘constant conjunction’ of events).
Constuctionism/constructivism
phenomena of the social world (including all our knowledge of it) are not objective entities but are constructs of the mind arrived at (constructed) through social interaction
Interpretivism
humans though part of the natural world, are fundamentally different from other natural phenomena
Possess both self-consciousness and language
understand how language is used to construct a set of meanings in relation to some specific feature of human existence
Qualitative research adopts a broadly constructivist/interpretivist approach to the social world, although it does not embody a single unified method of approach
collection or interpretation of linguistic materials (texts) that can be subjected to analysis and interpreted for information about how social meanings are constructed in particular situations
Thematic Analysis
a method for identifying, analysing and reporting patterns (themes) within data
Braun and Clarke: a useful basic method
it works with a wide range of research questions;
it can be used to analyse different types of data;
it works with large or small data sets;
it can be applied to produce data-driven or theory-driven analyses.
Decisions
What counts as a theme?
captures something important about the data in relation to the research question, and represents some level of patterned response or meaning within the data set
Inductive vs. deductive thematic analysis
inductive – data driven
deductive – theory driven
Decisions
Semantic/explicit level or a latent/interpretive level
Semantic: progression from description to interpretation
Six phases of thematic analysis:
Familiarisation with the data: reading and re-reading the data and noting any analytic observations
Coding: not simply a method of data reduction, it is also an analytic process; codes capture both the semantic and conceptual reading of the data
Searching for themes: A theme is a coherent and meaningful pattern in the data relevant to the research question (coding your codes)
Six phases of thematic analysis:
Reviewing themes: checking that the themes ‘work’ in relation to both the coded extracts and the full data set
Defining and naming themes
Writing up: weaving together the analytic narrative and vivid data extracts to tell a reader a coherent and persuasive story about the data, contextualising it in relation to the existing literature.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77-101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Read
Qualitative Research Practical
Michael Cleary-Gaffney
Louise
https://vimeo.com/31849334
https://vimeo.com/31849400
https://vimeo.com/31849506
ALEXANDER
https://vimeo.com/31840943
https://vimeo.com/31841311
https://vimeo.com/31841373
https://vimeo.com/31841735
Qualitative Research – Your Practical Report
Select a minimum of 5 pages (max. 10 pages) from each of the transcripts (Alexander’s interview and Louise’s interview)
Conduct an inductive thematic analysis on the data
Possible question - What is the meaning of friendship?
Write up a report based on your analysis (maximum word limit 2500)
Submission deadline – 23rd March
Discuss Braun and Clarke’s (2006) six phases – key challenges
Theorising friendship
Theory of reciprocal altruism (exchange theory)
Alliance hypothesis – DeScioli and Kurzban (2009)
Brooks, R. (2007). Friends, peers and higher education. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 28(6), 693-707. doi: 10.1080/01425690701609912
Emslie, C., Hunt, K., & Lyons, A. (2013). The role of alcohol in forging and maintaining friendships amongst Scottish men in midlife. Health Psychology, 32(1), 33-41. doi: 10.1037/a0029874
Lydon, J. E., Jamieson, D. W., & Holmes, J. G. (1997). The meaning of social interactions in the transition from acquaintanceship to friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 536-548. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.536
Becker, J. A. H., Johnson, A. J., Craig, E. A., Gilchrist, E. S., Haigh, M. M., & Lane, L. T. (2009). Friendships are flexible, not fragile: Turning points in geographically-close and long-distance friendships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 26(4), 347-369. doi: 10.1177/0265407509344310
Qualitative studies on friendship
Chasin, C. J. D., & Radtke, H. L. (2012). “Friend Moments”: A Discursive Study of Friendship. Qualitative Research in Psychology. doi: 10.1080/14780887.2011.606068
Dupuis-Blanchard, S., Neufeld, A., & Strang, V. R. (2009). The Significance of Social Engagement in Relocated Older Adults. Qualitative Health Research, 19(9), 1186-1195. doi: 10.1177/1049732309343956
Tillmann-Healy, L. M. (2003). Friendship as Method. Qualitative Inquiry, 9(5), 729-749. doi: 10.1177/1077800403254894
Structure of the report
Abstract; Introduction; Method*; Results*; Discussion
Appendices (not included in word count) – include your annotated interview transcripts
Method
Include the following subheadings:
Participants (one male, one female University student....)
The interviews were conducted in Spring 2008 at Liverpool John Moores University by Tanya Corker and Alasdair Gordon-Finlayson. The aim of the interviews was to enable participants to talk at length about their experiences of friendships.
Nationality is provided in the videos and transcripts.
Student population so I presume 20-25 years.
Ethical considerations
participants are identifiable
Participants were explicitly informed that their anonymity and confidentiality could not be maintained
Made aware of right to withdraw consent and withdraw data prior to it being made available for research purposes
Given opportunity to review materials prior to publication and to edit or withdraw data
Method – subheadings (continued)
Data Collection (Semi-structured interviews: aim to enable participants to talk about their experiences of friendship; data had already been transcribed into text)
Data Analysis – theoretical assumptions; own epistemological approach/assumptions
Data Analysis Procedure – (refer to criteria for text selection and identify location of text for analysis; outline the actual process)
Taleporos, G., & McCabe, M. P. (2002). Body image and physical disability - personal perspectives. Social Science & Medicine, 54, 971-980.
“....a number of factors have affected this research, primarily the first author’s life experiences with a congenital physical disability. Over recent years, through personal struggles and through intimate discussions with his physically disabled friends and colleagues, the first author has developed a number of assumptions and hypotheses about body image, physical disability and society. These include that body image is a problematic issue for people with disabilities and that social attitudes towards physical difference are largely unfavourable. Furthermore, as a disability activist and a proponent of the social model of disability, the first author regards social forces as having a central role in shaping the experience of physical disability.”
Hussain, D., & Bhushan, B. (2012). Posttraumatic Growth Experiences among Tibetan Refugees: A Qualitative Investigation. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 10(2), 204-216
“In qualitative research, beliefs and assumptions of the researcher may influence the interpretation of data. Therefore, it is important that we make statements about our own beliefs and assumptions associated to this study. The data for this study were collected as a part of my (first author) doctoral research. I was attracted to study PTG among Tibetan refugees because of my first hand encounter with many Tibetan refugees. I was intrigued by the thriving refugee communities and the pervasive impact of Buddhist culture in all aspects of their lives. It was evident that despite multiple traumatic experiences, Tibetan refugees continue to survive and attempt to heal through efforts to reconnect with their heritage. Therefore, interviews were designed to bring out these aspects.”
Results Section
Present themes with supportive quotes
Organize findings according to chronology—steps of a decision making process
Use case studies to illustrate cross -cutting themes cutting themes within a single case
Everything we do involves some level of quantification
Identifying a salient themes/pattern involves seeing something over and over again
Anytime we take raw data and put it into categories and look for patterns it implies a numbered nature of these look for patterns.
In qualitative research, the text (words of our informants) is our data, and thus we usually end up presenting some direct quotations.
The challenge is how to identify what are illustrative quotes to present and how to balance the presentation of quotes with some interpretation of findings.
30
Avoid using too many quotes
Often difficult to select one illustrative quote, so authors end up including too many quotes.
There needs to be some synthesis/ interpretation of quotes in addition to the raw data.
Readers lose sight of the main point, when there are too many quotes
How to choose an illustrative quote
There are no rules about what makes a good quote to include in a paper
Often you have a selection of many quotes that represent a specific theme
Choose quotes that most concisely represent the idea or theme you are trying to express
Words or phrases that are memorable often make the best quotations
Don’t choose quotations that are extreme outliers, unless this is that point your are trying to make in the paper
Results
Supported by illustrative quotations – “choose particularly vivid extracts which capture the essence of the point”
Ellipses/ellipsis points ( . . . ) are to be used only to represent deleted words or phrases, and not pauses in speech.
Set quotations of fewer than 40 words within regular sentences. Set quotations of 40 or more words as block quotes. (Use Word’s “Word Count” feature.)
Indent block quotes by ½ inch (approximately 1.3 cm.) from the left margin only. (Use Word’s “Format > Paragraph” feature to create the indentation.)
Type your quotations in 12-point Times New Roman font, double spaced. Do not use italics.
“Extracts need to be embedded within an analytic narrative that compellingly illustrates the story that you are telling about your data, and your analytic narrative needs to go beyond description of the data, and make your argument in relation to the research question” (Braun & Clarke, 2006)
Discussion
Aim of the study, the research question asked and how this was achieved
Links are made to previous research and some sort of critical debate is offered
Offering substantial evidence that such links really do exist. Evidence might be linked to theories or
Discussion
The discussion should stick to the findings. It is sometimes tempting for the researcher to speculate about the meaning of his or her findings or to try to ‘get inside the head’ of the respondent and somehow ‘interpret’ what that respondent meant.
Discussion
The best approach is to both present the findings in a flat and factual way and to offer a discussion that never strays further than the limits of the data. However, it is also important that the findings are discussed and that the writer does not produce merely a bald account of some of his or her findings.
Discussion
Summarise the findings and provide applications to of the work
Discuss the strengths of the work, the limitations and future studies.
Some critique of the qualitative approach is acceptable however extensive criticism, to the detriment of engagement with the spirit and ethos of the approach, is not appropriate
Discussion
Suggestions for future research should relate to future qualitative research and/or research derived from findings of qualitative approach. Suggestions which rely heavily on reference to quantitative approaches as the only viable approach should be avoided.
Data:
Louise’s Interview
Alexander’s interview
How did you choose your course of study?
How do employers choose among job candidates?
Would you trust any standardized battery of assessments sufficiently to choose your partner based solely on the results?
Task......
1 = very poor to 5 = excellent
How would you rate the job ‘nurse’ ?
Explain your answer to a person sitting next to you
Compare the rating to the verbal explanation
Positivism and the ‘standard view’ of science
‘the external world itself determines absolutely the one and only correct view that can be taken of it, independent of the process or circumstances of the viewing’ (Kirk and Miller, 1986: 14).
objective knowledge;
understanding that is unbiased and impartial;
based on a view from ‘the outside’
Nagel (1986) - the ‘view from nowhere’
Features of the positivist approach
objective knowledge (facts) can be gained from direct experience or observation, and is the only knowledge available to science; invisible or theoretical entities are rejected;
science separates facts from values; it is value-free
purpose of science is to develop universal causal laws by finding empirical regularities where two or more things appear together (a ‘constant conjunction’ of events).
Constuctionism/constructivism
phenomena of the social world (including all our knowledge of it) are not objective entities but are constructs of the mind arrived at (constructed) through social interaction
Interpretivism
humans though part of the natural world, are fundamentally different from other natural phenomena
Possess both self-consciousness and language
understand how language is used to construct a set of meanings in relation to some specific feature of human existence
Qualitative research adopts a broadly constructivist/interpretivist approach to the social world, although it does not embody a single unified method of approach
collection or interpretation of linguistic materials (texts) that can be subjected to analysis and interpreted for information about how social meanings are constructed in particular situations
Thematic Analysis
a method for identifying, analysing and reporting patterns (themes) within data
Braun and Clarke: a useful basic method
it works with a wide range of research questions;
it can be used to analyse different types of data;
it works with large or small data sets;
it can be applied to produce data-driven or theory-driven analyses.
Decisions
What counts as a theme?
captures something important about the data in relation to the research question, and represents some level of patterned response or meaning within the data set
Inductive vs. deductive thematic analysis
inductive – data driven
deductive – theory driven
Decisions
Semantic/explicit level or a latent/interpretive level
Semantic: progression from description to interpretation
Six phases of thematic analysis:
Familiarisation with the data: reading and re-reading the data and noting any analytic observations
Coding: not simply a method of data reduction, it is also an analytic process; codes capture both the semantic and conceptual reading of the data
Searching for themes: A theme is a coherent and meaningful pattern in the data relevant to the research question (coding your codes)
Six phases of thematic analysis:
Reviewing themes: checking that the themes ‘work’ in relation to both the coded extracts and the full data set
Defining and naming themes
Writing up: weaving together the analytic narrative and vivid data extracts to tell a reader a coherent and persuasive story about the data, contextualising it in relation to the existing literature.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77-101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Read
Qualitative Research Practical
Michael Cleary-Gaffney
Louise
https://vimeo.com/31849334
https://vimeo.com/31849400
https://vimeo.com/31849506
ALEXANDER
https://vimeo.com/31840943
https://vimeo.com/31841311
https://vimeo.com/31841373
https://vimeo.com/31841735
Qualitative Research – Your Practical Report
Select a minimum of 5 pages (max. 10 pages) from each of the transcripts (Alexander’s interview and Louise’s interview)
Conduct an inductive thematic analysis on the data
Possible question - What is the meaning of friendship?
Write up a report based on your analysis (maximum word limit 2500)
Submission deadline – 23rd March
Discuss Braun and Clarke’s (2006) six phases – key challenges
Theorising friendship
Theory of reciprocal altruism (exchange theory)
Alliance hypothesis – DeScioli and Kurzban (2009)
Brooks, R. (2007). Friends, peers and higher education. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 28(6), 693-707. doi: 10.1080/01425690701609912
Emslie, C., Hunt, K., & Lyons, A. (2013). The role of alcohol in forging and maintaining friendships amongst Scottish men in midlife. Health Psychology, 32(1), 33-41. doi: 10.1037/a0029874
Lydon, J. E., Jamieson, D. W., & Holmes, J. G. (1997). The meaning of social interactions in the transition from acquaintanceship to friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 536-548. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.536
Becker, J. A. H., Johnson, A. J., Craig, E. A., Gilchrist, E. S., Haigh, M. M., & Lane, L. T. (2009). Friendships are flexible, not fragile: Turning points in geographically-close and long-distance friendships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 26(4), 347-369. doi: 10.1177/0265407509344310
Qualitative studies on friendship
Chasin, C. J. D., & Radtke, H. L. (2012). “Friend Moments”: A Discursive Study of Friendship. Qualitative Research in Psychology. doi: 10.1080/14780887.2011.606068
Dupuis-Blanchard, S., Neufeld, A., & Strang, V. R. (2009). The Significance of Social Engagement in Relocated Older Adults. Qualitative Health Research, 19(9), 1186-1195. doi: 10.1177/1049732309343956
Tillmann-Healy, L. M. (2003). Friendship as Method. Qualitative Inquiry, 9(5), 729-749. doi: 10.1177/1077800403254894
Structure of the report
Abstract; Introduction; Method*; Results*; Discussion
Appendices (not included in word count) – include your annotated interview transcripts
Method
Include the following subheadings:
Participants (one male, one female University student....)
The interviews were conducted in Spring 2008 at Liverpool John Moores University by Tanya Corker and Alasdair Gordon-Finlayson. The aim of the interviews was to enable participants to talk at length about their experiences of friendships.
Nationality is provided in the videos and transcripts.
Student population so I presume 20-25 years.
Ethical considerations
participants are identifiable
Participants were explicitly informed that their anonymity and confidentiality could not be maintained
Made aware of right to withdraw consent and withdraw data prior to it being made available for research purposes
Given opportunity to review materials prior to publication and to edit or withdraw data
Method – subheadings (continued)
Data Collection (Semi-structured interviews: aim to enable participants to talk about their experiences of friendship; data had already been transcribed into text)
Data Analysis – theoretical assumptions; own epistemological approach/assumptions
Data Analysis Procedure – (refer to criteria for text selection and identify location of text for analysis; outline the actual process)
Taleporos, G., & McCabe, M. P. (2002). Body image and physical disability - personal perspectives. Social Science & Medicine, 54, 971-980.
“....a number of factors have affected this research, primarily the first author’s life experiences with a congenital physical disability. Over recent years, through personal struggles and through intimate discussions with his physically disabled friends and colleagues, the first author has developed a number of assumptions and hypotheses about body image, physical disability and society. These include that body image is a problematic issue for people with disabilities and that social attitudes towards physical difference are largely unfavourable. Furthermore, as a disability activist and a proponent of the social model of disability, the first author regards social forces as having a central role in shaping the experience of physical disability.”
Hussain, D., & Bhushan, B. (2012). Posttraumatic Growth Experiences among Tibetan Refugees: A Qualitative Investigation. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 10(2), 204-216
“In qualitative research, beliefs and assumptions of the researcher may influence the interpretation of data. Therefore, it is important that we make statements about our own beliefs and assumptions associated to this study. The data for this study were collected as a part of my (first author) doctoral research. I was attracted to study PTG among Tibetan refugees because of my first hand encounter with many Tibetan refugees. I was intrigued by the thriving refugee communities and the pervasive impact of Buddhist culture in all aspects of their lives. It was evident that despite multiple traumatic experiences, Tibetan refugees continue to survive and attempt to heal through efforts to reconnect with their heritage. Therefore, interviews were designed to bring out these aspects.”
Results Section
Present themes with supportive quotes
Organize findings according to chronology—steps of a decision making process
Use case studies to illustrate cross -cutting themes cutting themes within a single case
Everything we do involves some level of quantification
Identifying a salient themes/pattern involves seeing something over and over again
Anytime we take raw data and put it into categories and look for patterns it implies a numbered nature of these look for patterns.
In qualitative research, the text (words of our informants) is our data, and thus we usually end up presenting some direct quotations.
The challenge is how to identify what are illustrative quotes to present and how to balance the presentation of quotes with some interpretation of findings.
30
Avoid using too many quotes
Often difficult to select one illustrative quote, so authors end up including too many quotes.
There needs to be some synthesis/ interpretation of quotes in addition to the raw data.
Readers lose sight of the main point, when there are too many quotes
How to choose an illustrative quote
There are no rules about what makes a good quote to include in a paper
Often you have a selection of many quotes that represent a specific theme
Choose quotes that most concisely represent the idea or theme you are trying to express
Words or phrases that are memorable often make the best quotations
Don’t choose quotations that are extreme outliers, unless this is that point your are trying to make in the paper
Results
Supported by illustrative quotations – “choose particularly vivid extracts which capture the essence of the point”
Ellipses/ellipsis points ( . . . ) are to be used only to represent deleted words or phrases, and not pauses in speech.
Set quotations of fewer than 40 words within regular sentences. Set quotations of 40 or more words as block quotes. (Use Word’s “Word Count” feature.)
Indent block quotes by ½ inch (approximately 1.3 cm.) from the left margin only. (Use Word’s “Format > Paragraph” feature to create the indentation.)
Type your quotations in 12-point Times New Roman font, double spaced. Do not use italics.
“Extracts need to be embedded within an analytic narrative that compellingly illustrates the story that you are telling about your data, and your analytic narrative needs to go beyond description of the data, and make your argument in relation to the research question” (Braun & Clarke, 2006)
Discussion
Aim of the study, the research question asked and how this was achieved
Links are made to previous research and some sort of critical debate is offered
Offering substantial evidence that such links really do exist. Evidence might be linked to theories or
Discussion
The discussion should stick to the findings. It is sometimes tempting for the researcher to speculate about the meaning of his or her findings or to try to ‘get inside the head’ of the respondent and somehow ‘interpret’ what that respondent meant.
Discussion
The best approach is to both present the findings in a flat and factual way and to offer a discussion that never strays further than the limits of the data. However, it is also important that the findings are discussed and that the writer does not produce merely a bald account of some of his or her findings.
Discussion
Summarise the findings and provide applications to of the work
Discuss the strengths of the work, the limitations and future studies.
Some critique of the qualitative approach is acceptable however extensive criticism, to the detriment of engagement with the spirit and ethos of the approach, is not appropriate
Discussion
Suggestions for future research should relate to future qualitative research and/or research derived from findings of qualitative approach. Suggestions which rely heavily on reference to quantitative approaches as the only viable approach should be avoided.
Running head: WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE
A Qualitative Study of Women’s Self-Concept During Menopause
Leanne Kelly
17521573
Supervisor: Dr. Caoimhe Hannigan
Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Bachelor of Arts (Hons)
Degree in Psychology, Submitted to the National College of Ireland, March 2020.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE I
Submission of Thesis to Norma Smurfit Library, National College of Ireland
Student name: Leanne Kelly Student number: 17521573
School: School of Business Course: Psychology
Degree to be awarded: B.A. (Hons) in Psychology
Title of Thesis: A Qualitative Study of Women’s Self-Concept During Menopause
One hard bound copy of your thesis will be lodged in the Norma Smurfit Library and will be
available for consultation. The electronic copy will be accessible in TRAP (http://trap.ncirl.ie/),
the National College of Ireland’s Institutional Repository. In accordance with normal academic
library practice all theses lodged in the National College of Ireland Institutional Repository
(TRAP) are made available on open access.
I agree to a hard-bound copy of my thesis being available for consultation in the library. I also
agree to an electronic copy of my thesis being made publicly available on the National College
of Ireland’s Institutional Repository TRAP.
Signature of Candidate:
Leanne Kelly
For completion by the School:
The aforementioned thesis was received by______________________ Date: _____________
http://trap.ncirl.ie/
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE II
Submission of Thesis and Dissertation
National College of Ireland
Research Students Declaration Form
(Thesis/Author Declaration Form)
Name: Leanne Kelly
Student Number: 17521573
Degree for which thesis is submitted: B.A. (Hons) in Psychology
Material submitted for award
(a) I declare that the work has been composed by myself.
(b) I declare that all verbatim extracts contained in the thesis have been distinguished by
quotation marks and the sources of information specifically acknowledged.
(c) My thesis will be included in electronic format in the College Institutional Repository
TRAP (thesis reports and projects)
(d) I declare that no material contained in the thesis has been used in any other submission
for an academic award.
Signature of research student: Leanne Kelly
Date: 28/02/2020
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE III
Acknowledgements
Firstly, I would like to thank the participants who gave up their time to help me conduct this
research - I greatly appreciate it. I would like to thank my supervisor, Caoimhe, who has guided
and reassured me every step of the way, and all the staff at NCI who have provided me with the
tools to progress to this stage in my degree. My amazing Mam, my Dad, my Nanny, and my
wonderful boyfriend, Jordan, who have all put up with me through the trials and tribulations of
this research project. And last, but by no means least, my amazing friends – my hot girl scholars
– who have supported and motivated me every day without fail; I couldn’t have done it without
you all.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE IV
Abstract
Background and Aims: Much like puberty in adolescents, the mid-life transition to menopause
by women is characterized by many physical, social and psychological changes. Previous
research has investigated various aspects of women’s experience of and attitudes towards
menopause, but little qualitative research has examined women’s self-concept during this phase.
There is also limited research surrounding menopause in general within an Irish context.
Therefore, the aim of this research was to explore Irish women’s experience of the menopausal
transition and to understand if and how their experience has impacted their self-concept.
Method: Participants who had experienced symptoms associated with the natural menopause
transition in the previous two years were recruited via convenience sampling (N = 10). All
participants were Irish, and their ages ranged from 43-54 years (M = 49.70, SD = 3.65). Semi-
structured interviews were conducted with each participant which included questions relating to
various dimensions of women’s self-concept (e.g. self-esteem and body-image). The data were
analysed with adherence to Braun and Clarke’s (2006) six phases of thematic analysis. Results:
Four key themes were identified: (i) Loss of identity, womanhood and control, (ii) Negative self-
appraisal, (iii) Impact of menopause and self-concept on quality of life, (iv) Positive changes in
self-concept; a time for self-compassion and self-acceptance. Conclusions: The experience of
menopause is unique to every woman. The negative impact that the experience can have on one’s
self-concept should be considered by health care practitioners to maximize support for women
during this significant developmental period.
Keywords: Menopause; self-concept; identity; self-image; body-image; thematic analysis
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE V
Table of Contents
A Qualitative Study of Women’s Self-Concept During Menopause ................................... 1
Methods............................................................................................................................. 13
Participants .................................................................................................................... 13
Data Collection ............................................................................................................. 14
Study Procedure ............................................................................................................ 15
Ethical Considerations .............................................................................................. 16
Data Analysis ................................................................................................................ 17
Data Analysis Procedure ............................................................................................... 17
Results ............................................................................................................................... 18
Loss of identity, womanhood and control ..................................................................... 19
Negative self-appraisal.................................................................................................. 23
Negative impact of menopause and self-concept on quality of life .............................. 26
Positive changes in self-concept; a time for self-compassion and self-acceptance ...... 29
Discussion ......................................................................................................................... 31
Strengths and Limitations ............................................................................................. 35
Implications and Future Research ................................................................................. 36
References ......................................................................................................................... 38
Appendices ........................................................................................................................ 49
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 1
A Qualitative Study of Women’s Self-Concept During Menopause
The human life cycle encompasses a wide range of biologically determined critical points
(Beyene, 1986). Much like puberty in adolescents, the mid-life transition into menopause by
women is characterized by many physical, social and psychological changes (Kumari, Stafford,
& Marmot, 2005). The beginning of what one would typically consider ‘menopause’, is known
as the perimenopausal transition; characterised by deviations in normal ovulatory cycles, and
menstrual irregularities, including the onset of menopausal symptoms (Speroff, 2002). The
typical age range for entry into this stage is 39-51 years, with an average duration of five years
(Treloar, 1981). Common symptoms that are experienced by women in this phase include hot
flashes, heavy and irregular periods, urinary incontinence and infections, vaginal atrophy,
reduced sexual function, sleep disturbances and cognitive disturbances (Woods & Mitchell,
2005). Menopause occurs with the loss of ovarian follicular function, resulting in the permanent
cessation of menstruation, which is clinically diagnosed when twelve consecutive months of
amenorrhea have occurred (Kahwati, Haigler, & Rideout, 2005). According to the longitudinal
Massachusetts Women’s Health Study (McKinlay, Brambilla, & Posner, 1992), menopause is
reached at an average age of 51 years, with an age range of 44-56 years (Treloar, 1974). The
transition to menopause may also increase the risk of some chronic conditions including
coronary heart disease and osteoporosis, with some epidemiological studies showing possible
associations with increased incidence of arthritis, Alzheimer’s disease, periodontal disease, skin
atrophy, and cancer (Greendale, Lee, & Arriola, 1999). Most women experience similar
menopausal symptoms, with approximately 85% reporting that they have experienced ≥1
symptom (McKinlay et al.,1992). In a sample of 6201 menopausal women from the US, 60%
reported seeking health care for their menopausal symptoms, with 50% reporting that they use
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 2
hormone replacement therapy (HRT) or HRT and alternative medicines combined (Williams et
al., 2007).
In addition to physiological symptoms, the menopausal transition is associated with a
range of psychological and social outcomes. It is important to note that menopause does not
occur in isolation from any social or psychological changes that may also occur at this phase in a
woman’s life (Deeks, 2003). Deeks (2003) stresses the importance of incorporating the context
of a woman’s life into our understanding of menopause, including due regard to her
psychological state, cultural and social background, the age at which menopause was reached,
etc. As previously mentioned, some women experience cognitive disturbances during this phase,
as well as depression, anxiety and irritability (George, 2002). However, to give meaning to the
concept of menopause, one must also consider co-occurring psychosocial factors such as
stressful life events, role demands, inadequate coping skills and possible past psychiatric
disorders (Avis, Brambilla, McKinlay, & Vass, 1994; Kaufert et al., 1992; Woods & Mitchell,
1997). For instance, studies have found that various psychosocial factors are linked with
depression during menopause, including self-concept, attitudes, social learning, coping skills and
changes in health status during their transition to menopause (Avis & McKinlay, 1991; Kaufert et
al., 1992). Both the physical and psychosocial implications of menopause have been found to
impact various aspects of women’s lives, including their relationships, their family and their
work (Hvas & Gannik, 2008; Parish, Faubion, Weinberg, Bernick, & Mirkin, 2019).
However, these impacts of the menopause vary widely among women due to differences
in social, cultural and individual attitudes and perceptions (Avis & Crawford, 2008; Beyene,
1986; Martin, Block, Sanchez, Arnaud, & Beyene, 1993; Richters, 1997; Robinson, 1996).
Beyene (1986) noted that if menopause is primarily a hormonal event, it would suffice to say that
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 3
all women would experience symptoms in the same way, which is not the case. Nevertheless, for
many years, menopause has been viewed from a narrow biomedical perspective, which has
proved problematic for many women and has led to the development of multiple negative
stereotypes surrounding menopause (Rostosky & Travis, 1996). In earlier Western society,
menopause was often defined as an estrogen-deficiency disease or ovarian dysfunction that
results in a number of illnesses (Greenblatt & Bruneteau, 1974). Rostosky and Travis (1996)
analysed articles published in various journals between 1984-1994 and found that the majority of
publications, whether in medical or psychology journals, are based directly or indirectly on a
biomedical model of menopause as a deficiency disease. Although menopause is now more
widely accepted as a normal and natural event in the life cycle of females, the ‘medicalization’ of
menopause in Western societies is still occurring, and has been thought to contribute to the high
prevalence of menopausal symptom reporting in Europe and North America (Beyene, 1986;
Freeman & Sherif, 2007), and an increase in the use of HRT (see Stuenkel et al., 2015).
However, due to the large variance among women’s experience of menopause, it is
suggested that the transition should be considered as a bio-psycho-social-cultural transition,
rather than being understood solely from a biomedical perspective (Hunter & Rendall, 2007).
The necessity for a shift in perspective is not limited to menopause; it has long since been
recognised that there are major limitations to the traditional biomedical model of health within
the domain of health care and general practice. Engel (1977) proposed the idea of a new model
of health and illness, acknowledging that the boundaries between ‘health’ and ‘disease’ will
never be clear and will never be fully explained by biological criteria, as they are diffused by
cultural, social and psychological factors.
Social Influences
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 4
The biomedical discourse was recently highlighted in Hvas and Gannik’s (2008)
exploratory research as something that women experiencing menopause relate to. In their study,
nearly all women referred to biomedical terms when speaking about their experience of
menopause (e.g. ‘a period of decline and decay’) even if they did not agree with this view. They
also identified six other discourses: (i) ‘Forever young’ discourse - menopause as a barrier to
youthfulness and longevity; (ii) Health-promoting discourse - menopause as a barrier to good
health and physical fitness, (iii) Consumer discourse - menopause as a process for which women
can make informed and educated choices with the help of health care practitioners, (iv)
Alternative discourse - menopause as a positive, natural process but also as an imbalance in the
body which can be treated with natural substances, (v) Feminist/critical discourse - menopause as
a natural, positive event which is medicalized by the medical profession, and (vi) Existential
discourse - menopause as a time of self-discovery and personal growth. These discourses
highlight the significance of how the menopausal transition is portrayed in a social context;
women’s experience of menopause is influenced by the way it is talked about within society,
which ultimately affects many aspects of their social and personal lives, including marriage,
work and family (Hvas & Gannik, 2008). Similarly, Gannon and Ekstrom (1993) conducted a
study with 372 women and 209 men who were randomly assigned to three groups. They were
asked to complete questionnaires on their attitudes towards either (i) three medical issues, (ii)
three life transitions or (iii) three aspects of ageing, all of which included menopause. Those who
were assigned to the medical issues group had significantly more negative attitudes towards
menopause than those who were assigned to the other two sociocultural paradigms. Again, this
highlights the significance of how menopause is spoken about and portrayed in a social context.
Cultural Influences
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 5
In addition to the aforementioned social influences, differences in women’s experience of
menopause can be seen cross-culturally. Beyene (1986) found that many Mayan women viewed
pregnancy as a dangerous and stressful experience, which according to interviews with their
medical personnel, was the greatest gynaecological problem among Mayan women - not
menopause. In contrast to their negative experience of childbearing, menopause was described
by these women as a new sense of freedom and youth, and a sense of relief from menstruation
and anxiety surrounding pregnancy, which resulted in improved sexual relationships with their
husbands. Similarly, Stewart (2003) found that many Guatemalan Mayan women are overjoyed
when they reach menopause due to the freedom and status that it is associated with. However, in
societies where fertility is highly valued, women tend to hold more negative attitudes towards
menopause (Khademi & Cooke, 2003). Researchers have suggested that the variance in
menopausal experience and symptoms can be attributed to variance in culture, as studies have
found that menopausal women in non-western societies tend to report fewer symptoms than
menopausal women in Europe and the United States (e.g. Dennerstein, 1996; Lock, 1994). It
should be noted however, that there appears to be a lack of research around Irish women’s
experience of menopause (Carolan, 2000). The main themes identified within Carolan’s (2000)
study were that women of high parity in rural Ireland experienced a shared sense of relief at
reaching menopause, particularly related to the end of their childbearing years; a sense of
acceptance of menopause as a natural event in a woman’s life cycle; and a sense of satisfaction
after successfully raising their families. Carolan (2000) recommends that further research is
needed to develop a more comprehensive understanding of Irish women’s experiences of
menopause. Hoga, Rodolpho, Gonçalves, and Quirino (2015) recently conducted a systematic
review of qualitative studies investigating women’s experience of natural menopause worldwide.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 6
Although only 24 studies met the inclusion criteria (N = 575), an indicator of a lack of research
in this area, the six primary findings were that menopause is a natural event in a woman’s life
that is closely associated with psychosocial events of midlife and aging; the physical and
emotional changes of menopause strongly affect women; menopause is a time characterized by
gains and losses; resilience is improved and coping strategies are adopted to enhance wellbeing;
health issues, family and marital relations, sociocultural backgrounds and the meanings
attributed to the women’s sex life determine sexual satisfaction during menopause; and that
women should be prepared and have their needs supported.
Individual Influences
In addition to social and cultural influences, individual perceptions and attitudes have
also been found to impact women’s experience of menopause and their psychological adjustment
during this phase. These individual differences are crucial for providing effective and tailored
care, which is why healthcare professionals should acquire a “capacity to understand the patient’s
inner world – the values she lives by, her thoughts, feelings and fears; her perception of the
[injury] and its effect on her life.” (McWhinney, 1997, p.64). Extensive research has been
conducted on women’s attitudes towards the menopausal transition. Women are more likely to
report menopausal symptoms - and suffer more from them - if they hold negative attitudes
towards the transition, than women who hold more positive attitudes (Avis, Crawford, &
McKinlay, 1997; Ayers, Forshaw & Hunter, 2010; Bloch, 2002; Sievert & Espinosa-Hernandez,
2003). By contrast, those who hold more positive attitudes towards menopause tend to view the
transition as a natural life process (Ayers et al., 2010; Dolińska-Zygmunt & Włodarczyk, 2012).
Women’s attitudes towards menopause have also been found to vary across stages of their
transition. For example, most women who are only beginning to experience symptoms of
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 7
menopause describe it very negatively (Marvan, Castillo-López, & Arroyo, 2013), while those
who are post-menopausal often describe the transition as a sense of relief and satisfaction (e.g.
Carolan, 2000). Brown, Brown, Judd, and Bryant (2017) also found that within a sample of 387
women at various stages of the menopause, those who reported themselves as postmenopausal
had significantly more positive cognitive and emotional representations of the menopause than
those who were premenopausal and perimenopausal. Longitudinal studies have shown that these
differences in attitudes are not static. For example, Busch, Barth-Olofsson, Rosenhagen and
Collins (2003) assessed 130 healthy women over the course of five years through both
quantitative and qualitative methods, finding that 57% held neutral beliefs about menopause,
31% had negative beliefs (pessimistic), and 12% had positive beliefs (optimistic), with negative
appraisals significantly relating to higher levels of symptom reporting. However, at the last
follow up, 67% of the women appraised menopause positively, compared with the previous 12%,
which was found to be associated with statements of personal growth among the previously
pessimistic appraisal group.
Associations have been found between attitudes towards menopause and well-being and
quality of life. Within a sample of 1503 Australian-born women at various stages of the
menopausal transition, Dennerstein, Smith and Morse (1994) found that menopausal status did
not significantly affect well-being, but well-being was significantly related to current health
status (psychosomatic symptoms and respiratory symptoms), history of premenstrual complaints,
overall health, interpersonal stress, and attitudes to ageing and to menopause. Despite these
variations, numerous studies have found that attitudes towards menopause, and not menopausal
status, are significantly related to well-being/quality of life (e.g. Dennerstein, Smith, & Morse,
1994; Dennerstein, 1996), with negative attitudes resulting in negative moods (Dennerstein,
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 8
1996) and maladaptive schemas, e.g. vulnerability/isolation, which affects well-being
(Hashemipoor, Jafari, & Zabihi, 2019). Well-being and quality of life have repeatedly been
associated with dimensions of self-concept such as self-esteem across various populations (e.g.
Diener & Diener, 2009; Mohanty & Sahoo, 2015; Ritchie, Sedikides, Wildschut, Arndt, &
Gidron, 2011; Sowislo & Orth, 2013). If attitudes and perceptions of menopause have been
shown to impact experience of menopause, well-being and quality of life, and these constructs
have been associated with self-concept and self-esteem, then it may be the case that self-concept
might be impacted by the menopause.
Menopause and Self-Concept
To understand adult development and ageing, many lifespan researchers have concluded
that the self is an important element which must be considered (Baltes & Baltes, 1990;
Whitbourne, 1985). According to humanistic theorist Carl Rogers (1959), the self-concept is
composed of one’s self-image, self-esteem or self-worth, and one’s ideal self. It is the product of
a collection of self-schemas, defined through self-evaluations, that makes up one’s overall self-
concept (Beck, Steer, Epstein, & Brown, 1990). At present, the majority of researchers adopt a
multidimensional perspective of self-concept, based on the multidimensional hierarchical model
of self-concept (Shavelson, Hubner, & Stanton, 1976), which incorporates various domains into
self-concept, e.g. social, emotional and academic contexts. Self-concept is often seen as a
component within the broad construct of identity (Adamson, Hartman, & Lyxell, 1999), which is
comprised of the individual’s mental representation of themselves. Individuals’ self-concept
develops and changes throughout the lifespan with exposure to sensory and behavioural
experience, physical body changes and societal norms (O’Brien, 1980; Pasquali, 1999). Identity
and self-concept have been studied across a range of populations, with a particular focus on
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 9
adolescents and, notably, their academic self-concept (e.g. von Keyserlingk, Becker, & Jansen,
2019). Most of these studies have utilized quantitative methodology, for example the Beck Self-
Concept Test (Beck et al., 1990). Across populations, self-concept clarity has been negatively
related to measures of psychological distress (e.g., anxiety and negative affect) and is associated
with an increase in subjective well-being (Campbell et al., 1996; De Cremer & Sedikides, 2005;
Lavallee & Campbell, 1995; Slotter, Gardner, & Finkel, 2010).
Shu, Luh, Li, and Lu (2007) investigated self-concept in a sample of Taiwanese women at
varying stages of menopause. A Chinese version of the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale (Fitts &
Roid, 1964) was used to measure physical self, moral-ethical self, psychological self, family self,
social self and academic attainment self. 92.5% of the women’s self-concept were within the
normal range, but slightly lower than that of the adult population of Taiwan, while 7.5% had an
abnormal score, scoring most poorly on measures of physical self and academic self. Low
physical self scores were a significant predictor of psychological and physiological menopause
symptoms, which led the authors to conclude that a positive self-concept is a protective factor for
psychological distress and physiological symptoms associated with menopause. Self-esteem and
other dimensions of self-concept were also found to predict life satisfaction in 40.3% of a sample
of 404 perimenopausal and postmenopausal women (Martínez, González-Arratia, Oudhof van
Barneveld, Domínguez-Espinosa, & Olivos-Rubio, 2012). Similarly, Castiglione, Licciardello,
and Rampullo (2015) found that women who hold a positive representation of the self, adapt
better to menopausal changes than those with negative representations of the self. From a
qualitative approach, Svenson (2005) interviewed ten menopausal women to investigate their
sense of self, as he notes that there is a lack of literature surrounding women’s identity
consolidation during this phase of life. By contrast to the findings proposed by Shu et al. (2007),
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 10
Svenson (2005) found that women’s experience of physical and emotional disruption can affect
their sense of self, with many participants describing changes in self-identity.
Body-image, a physical dimension of self-concept, can be defined as an individual’s
“internal, subjective representation of physical appearance and bodily experience” (Pruzinsky &
Cash, 1990, p.338) and is integral to one’s identity and self-concept. Body image attitudes are
closely linked with self-esteem, confidence, eating and exercise behaviours, emotional stability
and sexual behaviours (Cash, 1990; Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999),
however the influences of body image in specific life contexts are often ignored in research
(Pruzinsky & Cash, 1990), despite the growing prevalence of negative body image, especially
among women (Cash, 1997; Cash & Fleming, 2002). Beauty and youth are often seen as
synonymous in society (Alderson, 1991), which can result in the experience of body image
disturbances by women in their mid/older adult life. Vasomotor instability (e.g., hot flashes, night
sweats) in menopausal women can lead to irregularities in body image and can often make
women feel like they are not in control of their bodies (Chrisler & Ghiz, 1993). The bodily
changes that occur during a woman’s transition through menopause can alter the way they think
or feel about themselves and their body (Chrisler & Ghiz, 1993). Despite this, few empirical
studies have assessed body image among women experiencing menopause, particularly
perceptions of body image (Deeks & McCabe, 2001). However, Deeks and McCabe (2001) did
find that menopausal women within their sample were characterized by a negative body image
and experiencing negative emotions regarding the appearance and function of their bodies.
Walter (2000) used qualitative methods by interviewing a sample of 21 menopausal women and
uncovered a range of ideas of how menopause influences women’s sense of self; physically,
emotionally, socially, and cognitively. They identified an underlying theme of uncertainty
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 11
amongst menopausal women; many women spoke of feeling out of control, vulnerable, and
disconnected from their bodies. Perimenopausal and menopausal women have also been shown
to be less positive about their appearance and fitness and score lower on measures related to
attractiveness and the way women feel about the way they look, than premenopausal women
(Deeks & McCabe, 2001). However, as previously mentioned, it should be noted that this may
also be influenced by the broader process of ageing and other psychosocial factors.
Self-esteem is a dimension of self-concept which is related to self-awareness, emotions,
cognitions, behaviour, lifestyle and socio-economic factors (Greenberg, 2008). Typically, high
self-esteem is related to positive components such as optimism, psychological well-being and
successful coping (Harrington & Loffredo, 2001), which have been associated with decreases in
all-cause mortality (Chida & Steptoe, 2008). Those who experience low self-esteem are usually
more susceptible to anxious thoughts, depressed moods and increased health problems (Chedraui
et al., 2010). However, Chedraui et al. (2010) surprisingly found that in a sample of 149 women,
age, menopausal status and time since menopause onset were not predictive of self-esteem
scores. They suggest that self-esteem of menopausal women may be influenced by social factors
such as changing roles, co-occurring life events or dissatisfaction with one’s body image. Pérez-
López, Chedraui, Gilbert and Pérez-Roncero (2009) also suggest that low self-esteem may be
due to co-morbid conditions such as sleep disorders, depression, anxiety, elevated body weight,
eating disorders or musculoskeletal disorders. However, Pinquart and Sorensen (2001) argue that
self-esteem in the later stages of life should not be underestimated, as their meta-analysis found
that older women scored significantly lower on subjective well-being (e.g. happiness, self-
esteem, life satisfaction) than their male counterparts. Similarly, women’s experience of low self-
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 12
esteem is cause for concern as it has been highlighted as a risk factor for depression and feelings
of helplessness (Beutel, Glaesmer, Decker, Fischbeck, & Brähler, 2009).
Guérin, Goldfield and Prud’homme (2017) found that women transitioning through
menopause who scored higher on self-esteem, body image and general health perceptions were
more likely to experience greater positive psychological outcomes over a five year period,
implying that more focus on women’s self-concept may allow for a healthier psychological
adjustment during this period. Numerous studies have investigated dimensions of self-concept in
those who experienced premature menopause due to medical intervention (e.g. Liao, Wood, &
Conway, 2000; Shepard, 1990; Parlee, 1990; Pasquali, 1999), or have used quantitative methods
to assess self-concept (e.g. Heidari, Ghodusi, & Rafiei, 2017; Shu et al., 2007), but few studies
have assessed self-concept within women experiencing natural menopause from a qualitative
approach. Similarly, very little research has examined the self-concepts or experiences of
menopausal women within Irish populations (Carolan, 2000). As most of the research has
investigated women’s attitudes towards menopause itself (e.g. Avis et al., 1997; Ayers et al.,
2010; Brown et al., 2017; Busch et al., 2003; Khademi & Cooke, 2003; Marvan et al., 2013;
Sievert & Espinosa-Hernandez, 2003), the literature fails to capture women’s self-concept and
identity during this critical developmental period.
The Current Study
The current study will adopt an inductive approach to uncover any and all relevant
themes surrounding women’s self-concept. It will address the following question: How do
women describe their self-concept during their experience of natural menopause? The aim of this
research is to explore Irish women’s experience of the menopausal transition and to understand if
and how their experience has impacted their self-concept. Ultimately, this will lead to a greater
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 13
understanding of their experience and shape the way for interventions to promote more positive
developmental outcomes for women during this significant life transition.
Methods
Participants
Participants were recruited online via convenience sampling. Posts outlining the nature of
the study were placed in various women’s/menopause support groups on Facebook, e.g. The Irish
Menopause, and the researcher’s personal Facebook page, where potential eligible participants
were invited to email the researcher at the contact details provided, should they wish to partake.
Inclusion criteria for the current study were women over the age of 18 years, who have
experienced symptoms associated with the natural menopausal transition in the previous two
years, at the exclusion of women who were experiencing symptoms of early menopause due to
medical intervention (e.g. chemotherapy, hysterectomy). This distinction has been made due to
the notable differences in women’s experiences of natural and medically induced menopause.
Kaufert (1990) mentions that “methodologically speaking, the naturally and the artificially
menopausal should neither be combined in the same study population, nor should generalism be
made from one set of women to the other. Yet, both these practices are common” (p. 117).
There is a lack of agreement in the literature on appropriate sample size for qualitative
studies. The literature often specifies that a sufficient sample must be recruited to achieve data
saturation. Guest, Bunce, and Johnson (2006) define data saturation as ‘the point in data
collection and analysis when new information produces little or no change to the codebook’ (p.
65). Data saturation has often been described as the ‘gold standard’ for determining sample size
in qualitative research (e.g. Guest et al., 2006), however, few researchers agree on how or when
this saturation is achieved, as noted in a recent review by Braun and Clarke (2019). For example,
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 14
after conducting sixty in-depth interviews and conducting a thematic analysis on the data, Guest
et al. (2006) claim that saturation and variability occurred within the first twelve interviews. By
contrast, Eynon, Donnell, and Williams (2018) reported that data saturation had been achieved
after the analysis of eight interview transcripts. Braun and Clarke (2019) advise that researchers
should determine their sample size within the process of data collection, with due regard to the
adequacy (richness and complexity) of the data for addressing the research question.
For the current study, a total of 14 women demonstrated an interest in partaking.
However, due to cancellations, geographical distance, and lack of participants’ availability during
the time scale the interviews were due to take place, the final sample consisted of 10 women. Six
participants, including the participant who partook in the pilot study, were recruited via the
researcher’s personal Facebook page, while four were recruited from the specific support pages.
Participants ages ranged from 43-54 years (M = 49.70, SD = 3.65). All participants were Irish
and identified as female.
Data Collection
As this study is qualitative by design, semi-structured interviews were conducted by the
researcher on a one-to-one basis, to enable participants to explore their own experience of
menopause, without having to adhere to a rigid schedule. This broad schedule (See Appendix A)
was devised by the researcher with due regard to the proposed research question and aims, and
included both general and specific questions relating to women’s self-concept, self-image, self-
esteem, identity, body-image, etc. It includes open-ended questions and prompts to capture the
essence of participants’ experience. The schedule was used flexibly, with some questions being
omitted or altered depending on the context of each interview.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 15
As this interview schedule has not previously been used, a pilot study was conducted
with one participant to ensure that data relevant to the research question was obtained. The pilot
study was successful, and the data obtained was included for analysis. All interviews were
transcribed verbatim for analysis.
Study Procedure
After potential participants demonstrated an interest in participating via email, they were
forwarded the relevant details, such as an in-depth information sheet (See Appendix B) and
consent form (See Appendix C). Interviews were then arranged via email, at a time and place of
the participants’ convenience. Three interviews were held in a private room in the National
College of Ireland, while the other seven were arranged at alternative, suitable locations.
Upon meeting, each participant was briefed with reference to the information sheet and
given the opportunity to ask any questions. Participants were then asked to read through the
consent form provided and to answer the two demographic questions, age and gender, followed
by signing if they gave their informed consent to partake in the interview.
Once informed consent was obtained, the interviews proceeded. The interview schedule
was used to facilitate the interviews but based on the nature of the interaction and the semi-
structured approach, a flexible approach was used in terms of the sequence and adaptation of
questions.
Interviews were audio recorded via recording and transcription applications on two
mobile phones until the end of the interview. The interviews ranged in time from 11-43 minutes
(M = 25), excluding the time spent on briefing and debriefing participants. This large time range
was due to some participants not having as much to say about their experience, i.e. their self-
concept was not as affected as some of the other participants.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 16
Participants were fully debriefed after the interview, which included verbal
acknowledgement of their participation, a recap of the aims of the interview and their rights with
regard to withdrawal and access to data, further reassurance of their anonymity and
confidentiality, providing the opportunity to ask questions, and providing them with relevant
contact details of support forums/helplines in a printed debrief form (See Appendix D).
Ethical Considerations
The current study adhered to the ethical guidelines provided by the Psychological Society
of Ireland and the National College of Ireland. Ethical approval for this research was granted by
the National College of Ireland’s Psychology Department’s Undergraduate Ethics Committee.
Informed consent was obtained to ensure beneficence and to protect participants. The
purpose and nature of the research was made completely transparent to participants at the
recruitment and data collection stage and it was ensured that all participants fully understood
what their participation would involve. Additionally, participants were informed of their right to
withdraw consent and their right to access their data under Freedom of Information legislation,
which is also outlined in the information sheet, consent form and debriefing sheet.
To safeguard anonymity and confidentiality, any potentially identifiable information was
omitted from the data upon transcription, and audio recordings were immediately destroyed after
this process. These transcripts were stored on a personal computer to which only the researcher
had access. These documents were password-protected and stored under a unique participant
number that was assigned to each participant to protect their identity.
As the study explores participants’ subjective experiences, the topic of menopause may
be sensitive to some. The risk of emotional distress during the interview process was minimised
by restating the participants’ right to withdraw consent of participation at any point. Some
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 17
participants did become emotional during the interviews; these participants were offered breaks
and given the opportunity to finish the interview if they wished to do so. However, all
participants were happy to continue. A full debrief, as previously mentioned, was carried out
with each participant.
Data Analysis
As this is a qualitative piece of research, it is essential to acknowledge that the beliefs and
assumptions of the researcher can influence interpretation of data. With this in mind, it should be
noted that the researcher was interested in studying self-concept within menopausal women due
to the impact the experience had on some family members. It was apparent that some women
found the transition extremely unsettling and demonstrated a notable change in their self-
concept, while others found that it was a relatively easy and non-impactful experience.
Therefore, the interviews were designed in a way that allowed for the flexible exploration of
women’s self-concept during this phase of life.
Data Analysis Procedure
Once all the data was transcribed to text from the audio tapes, an inductive thematic
analysis was conducted. This is a process of identifying, analysing and reporting patterns/themes
within the data. As this type of analysis is independent of theory and epistemology (Braun &
Clarke, 2006), it fits well with the social constructivist approach which argues that knowledge is
subjective and consists of our own representations of reality (Burr, 2003). Rather than testing
theory, this research sought to generate data from which a greater understanding of the research
topic might arise. This inductive approach allows for an analysis that is data-driven. Themes
were primarily identified at a semantic level, with some allowance for analysis beyond the
surface of the data, i.e. underlying ideas or assumptions. The theoretical freedom of thematic
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 18
analysis allowed the researcher to adopt an essentialist approach, reporting experiences,
meanings and the realities of participants. Due to the subjective nature of the research, this type
of analysis is well-suited.
The data was analysed with adherence to Braun and Clarke’s (2006) six phases of
thematic analysis. To begin, the individual transcripts were read repeatedly to increase familiarity
and to begin searching for patterns within the data. Codes were then produced manually by
writing notes and highlighting patterns on the printed transcripts to identify interesting features
of the data and to sort them into meaningful groups. Next, the groups of codes were organized
into possible themes and sub-themes, with each theme capturing something important relating to
the research question. These groups were continuously revised and refined, which involved
combining and altering themes where needed, to warrant coherent distinctions between themes.
An initial thematic map was then produced manually (See Appendix E), in which themes were
defined; ensuring that they captured the essence of each concept and the reason why they are an
interesting feature of the data. The individual themes were then finalised with regard to all other
themes, ensuring that there is a comprehensible ‘story’ for each that fits the overall narrative of
the data.
Results
During the analysis of the interview transcripts, four key themes captured the essence of
the data: (i) Loss of identity, womanhood and control, (ii) Negative changes in self-concept, (iii)
Impact of menopause and self-concept on quality of life, (iv) Positive changes in self-concept; a
time for self-compassion and self-acceptance. See Appendix E for an illustrative map of these
themes and the emerging ideas within them.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 19
Loss of identity, womanhood and control
“I'm losing my womanhood, I'm losing my fertility, I'm losing everything.” (P1)
Women’s identity and sense of control appeared to be very impacted during their
transition to menopause. Various complex elements comprise this theme, including significant
changes to self-image and identity, the impacts of loss of fertility and loss of womanhood, a
sense of remorse upon ageing, and an overall feeling of loss of control.
Many participants described feeling that they had lost who they were before their
menopausal transition: “You're not who you used to be. So, you're losing that self-identity, your
own identity.” (P1). Many women described significant changes to their self-concept and
identity: “I just became pretty much like a shell, like a shell of myself (...) I was nearly forcing
myself to be human.” (P6) Participant 1 also felt like others didn’t view her as a person anymore
and described a new feeling of invisibility: “I just felt I was invisible. I just felt like I was a
nobody.” (P1). This loss of identity was captured in the way many women spoke about being
“back to” themselves at the end of perimenopause/reaching menopause: “I'm back to my old self
again.” (P4) Significant changes in identity can also be seen in women who described themselves
before and after their use of HRT. When Participant 6 began using HRT, she described herself as
being “restored”:
I took a chance on a doctor, a very expensive doctor. And he listened to me and said to
me, you're not mad, you need hormones. That was it, I haven't gone back since. And
[name in third person] as we know her and always was, was back (...) it's like as if I’d just
been reset.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 20
When asked how she viewed herself before her menopausal transition, her reply was:
A perfectly normal human being, like I probably am now - I don't know if I come across
as normal. I was active, I was busy, I was always really good at my job, I was articulate, I
loved to have a laugh, I never stopped talking. I'm back to that now.
This clearly demonstrates the large impact that menopause has had on this participant’s identity,
as her self-image before menopause was completely different to her self-image during her
menopausal transition.
A large part of the impact on women’s identity was the feeling of losing their
womanhood. Participant 1 in particular described intense feelings of loss surrounding her
womanhood:
And obviously, the way you think in your head about who you are, you don't rate yourself
as a person or as a woman anymore (…) I felt as a woman, I was losing being a woman,
basically, if I was losing my periods and I was losing all of that end of things.
This participant also felt apprehensive when speaking to men about menopause for fear that they
would view her as “less of a woman.” Similarly, Participant 3 described feeling more like a man
during her transition:
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 21
There are days I felt like I was turning into a man like, you know. Cos I’m putting on
weight, I’m cross, sometimes I’m grumpy and can’t tolerate the child (…) I felt like I was
turning into a man.
Participant 5 described the loss of fertility experienced with menopause as having a
significant impact on them:
I think there was a grief, there's a grieving process that I think I went through. Losing my
fertility, I can't have any more kids, and feeling like you're just fading into the
background that you're not young and vibrant anymore - I mean there is that fear of
aging. And when you don't get your period, you go right that’s it, I'm moving on to- it's a
different phase.
This participant described the loss of her fertility as the biggest impact of menopause for her,
particularly because she “had such a hard time having kids” previously. By contrast, the majority
of participants failed to mention this aspect of the transition or described the loss of their periods
as being non-impactful. For example, Participant 8 stated that she was not impacted by the loss
of her fertility:
I know people, you know, kind of nearly mourn it because it’s the end of their
childbearing years, but I wasn't having any more children anyways. So, I just looked at it
as being rid of a nuisance basically.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 22
Other participants said things like “the best thing was to get rid of it” (P9) and that “it’s going to
happen whether you stress and worry or not” (P10), highlighting their indifference to this
change.
Accompanying these effects on women’s self-concept was an overriding feeling of loss of
control. Some participants described a lack of control over intrusive thoughts and intense mood
swings: “I knew they were irrational, but I couldn’t stop them.” (P1). However, it seems that the
lack of control is mostly described in terms of the overall experience of menopause itself, with
some participants describing the feeling of being “trapped,” highlighting their passive role in the
transition. A link can be seen between the feelings of loss of control and the view taken by some
participants that menopause is a marker of age: “The most important part of my life was gone
was done and I was, in a sense, not in control of my life anymore.” (P1) Some participants
described menopause as a period of “decay”, “decline” or a “natural progression to death”, which
evidently impacted their self-concept: “People used to say to me, ‘ah no you're too young.’ I still
think that I'm very young, you know what I mean? And I'm not probably old enough to be going
through the menopause. That impacted me.” (P2) Many other participants noted the impact that
their “young” age had on their experience and self-concept:
I think my age, full stop, has been the trickiest part of this whole thing, including the
doctors not listening because of my age. If I had have walked into any doctor at age 49/50
and said I'm having hot flushes at night, straight away they’d know what it was. 37/38 -
no you're mad, have an anti-anxiety tablet.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 23
Although most participants made this association between menopause and ageing, some did not
share this view: “I don’t look at it as getting old because, I'm just me. I’m not different.” (P8).
The lack of control over menopause and ageing in this case did not impact their self-concept.
Negative self-appraisal
“My mental thoughts were hugely affected by it, as much, if not more a lot of the time,
than the physical symptoms.” (P1)
The data illustrated a predominantly negative impact of the menopause on women’s self-
concept. This included negative descriptions of self-image and body-image, low self-esteem and
confidence, shame and preoccupation with others’ opinions.
Almost all participants described themselves in negative terms at some point during the
interviews. The language participants used to describe themselves was striking and consistent.
Some frequently used descriptions include: “crazy”, “headcase”, “wired”, “raving lunatic”,
“going off my head”, “losing my marbles”, and “losing my mind.” Most participants described
this sense of “craziness” as being a part of the transition: “There's a certain madness that seems
to come along with menopause.” (P7) It had a significant impact on some participants: “I
believed that I had some crazy psychiatric thing and that I was done. I was going to end up
locked up in an asylum like that's what I genuinely believed. I constantly doubted myself.” (P6)
Feelings of hopelessness and worthlessness were also shared by some participants,
particularly for Participant 6 who was very negatively impacted by the experience:
I was suicidal at one point, probably for most of it (…) there was no reason whatsoever to
live, because every moment of every day was pure agony physically and mentally. The
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 24
only peace I ever got was when I was asleep (…) I still don't really get my head around
how I'm still here or why did I hang on? (P6)
These feelings of worthlessness were also experienced by Participant 1, who had thoughts of,
“What's the point of being here? Nobody really needs me anyway.” She explained that she
suffered from “a depression” when she started going through menopause. Other participants felt
feelings of worthlessness at a less extreme level, describing that they felt like a “failure” due to
their inability to carry out every-day tasks as a result of tiredness.
Many participants provided an insight into their body-image during this phase, with the
majority describing their appearance as undesirable. The largest impact of body-image on
women’s self-concept appeared to be weight gain, with women describing their experiences of
not being able to lose weight in this phase of their life as “disheartening,” and that it affected
their confidence. Some women’s body-image concerns were not related to their weight,
particularly those who reported having recently lost weight. However, these participants were
more concerned with other physical changes to their hair, skin, etc.: “I didn't like the changes
physically in me of getting older. And menopause has a lot to do with that as well. And it just has
a huge mental impact.” (P1) Some participants described the negative self-talk that they
experienced: “It can be raging like, you know, the self-talk, the self-critique. You know, god your
hair, your face, your skin, your body's falling apart. You know, ‘ugh’, that that sense of ‘UGH’.”
(P5) The aforementioned feeling of loss of womanhood was also seen in the way women spoke
about their appearance: “Someone said to me the other day, ‘Have you got hair on your face?,’
and I’m like, I’m totally turning into a man.” (P3) Interestingly, women who had previous issues
with their bodies did not seem to be affected in this regard when going through menopause:
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 25
“Sure I never would have been really, body-confident anyway so I don't think it's any worse than
it was before.” (P10)
These negative descriptions of themselves came hand in hand with low self-esteem and
confidence, embarrassment, shame, and a preoccupation with other people’s opinions. Some
participants reported feelings of embarrassment and shame due to their experience of
menopause:
And I suppose again, it's a bit of a shame in yourself, you're a bit shameful or, you know,
you feel shame that you're going through it. Why should you feel shame for something
that happens naturally to every single woman? But I think there is a level of shame
attached to it. (P1)
Embarrassment was experienced in the workplace by some participants who described the
experience of hot flashes:
I had day sweats where I'd be in a meeting or whatever and this sweat would just start
pouring down my face and people would think I was going to get sick, and that started to
become embarrassing (…) they're looking and you can see them looking at the beads of
sweat, so that doesn't do your self-confidence any good, or to be constantly feeling that
you have to have a shower. (P10)
Participant 1 in particular described herself as a very confident person before her
menopausal transition, so this loss of confidence was something that was new to her. Other
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 26
participants also mentioned that they just “weren’t happy” with themselves or “didn’t like”
themselves during their transition. This also resulted in women ruminating over other’s opinions:
“And I remember thinking to myself, I know they're looking at me thinking I'm off my head and
I even think I'm off my head so why wouldn't they think I'm off my head.” (P1) Most participants
said that they would only speak to close friends about certain aspects of menopause, mainly the
“mental aspect”, for fear of judgement by others. Participant 3 even commented saying that it’s
best to explain to people what is going on, otherwise “they’re thinking you definitely have lost
it.” On the other hand, many participants said that they did not care much about others’ opinions:
“I wouldn’t really mind how other people see me, that wouldn’t bother me.” (P2) This highlights
the striking individual differences in women’s experiences and self-concept.
Negative impact of menopause and self-concept on quality of life
“I had no quality of life.” (P1)
The experience of menopause and its impact on the participants’ self-concept was made
apparent in the way they spoke about its effect on different aspects of their lives. Some
participants withdrew and isolated themselves, missed out on opportunities, resorted to alcohol
as a coping mechanism, and spoke about the impact on family, relationships and work.
Many participants mentioned that they began to isolate themselves during this phase of
their life due to the impact of physical symptoms (e.g. “If I knew that it was going to be very
warm somewhere, I would probably choose not to go.” (P1)) and due to their own wish to be
alone:
I did distance myself from certain situations and going to certain places, and maybe not
necessarily over the physical end of it, but possibly more in my own head I just didn't
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 27
want to be with other people. (…) I can remember making conscious calls and excuses of
why not to go to places and why it'd be better to stay at home. (P1)
Other women explained that they didn’t feel like they were “good company” because they were
feeling “down” and therefore “didn’t want to be mixing with people.” (P3) As a result, some
participants mentioned that they missed out on certain opportunities and life events, like
Participant 6 who described herself as having “missed out on really good years”:
I missed out on four years of my life. I didn't go out. I functioned, sort of. I went to work
because I had to and that was it like I’d no social life, I’d no nothing. All I ever wanted
was the bed.
This participant started resorting to alcohol in order to “function,” saying she “was held
together by a bottle of white wine” to get her through important family events. Another
participant, who said she just wanted to “keep away from people” described herself as using
alcohol as a reward to make her feel comfortable: “I think it was just my comfort zone at the
time, and it helped me through.” (P7) However, she reveals that she “could have gone down a
very slippery slope of drinking a lot.”
These factors appeared to impact on women’s family life. Most participants expressed
that their families “got the brunt of” the impact of the menopause. Intense feelings of guilt could
be seen, particularly in Participant 1 and Participant 6, who became upset when talking about the
impact of the experience on their family:
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 28
I think if you were to ask my family what I was like to live with back then they would
probably turn around and say you were a nightmare. And it probably was a nightmare and
it probably was horrible for them. But you can't turn back the clock, you can only
apologize and say I wasn't in my right mind or- [becomes visibly upset] (P1)
I feel the biggest impact for me would have been my daughter. It was always the guilt.
Always a huge guilt. I always found it so hard. And also, because of what was going on I
had no idea, I really didn't know what was wrong with me (…) I didn't have any
confirmation from any doctors and a part of me believed I was very ill and dying. And
that was the hardest part, I remember I used to put her to bed at night and I’d just feel so
sad because I didn't know if I was going to wake up the next morning. And it was hard
for her, cos I remember one time her asking, ‘Is mammy going to die?’ [becomes visibly
upset] (P6)
Relationships were also impacted by menopause and its impact on women’s self-concept
for some participants. Participant 3 described their partner as viewing them as a “nuisance”,
while Participant 7 explained how herself and her husband almost split up due to large arguments
which she attributes to the menopause, as well as finding his lack of support quite difficult to
accept: “I actually can't relate to my husband and say to him, ‘I don't feel good today,’ because he
can't handle it. So, I found it really difficult because I tried to talk to him.” (P7)
Some participants also spoke about how the symptoms of menopause and their lack of
confidence impacted their work and relationships with colleagues. Participant 1 described that
she feels her colleagues are still “on eggshells” with her because of the way she was before
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 29
taking HRT and that this has knocked her confidence. Similarly, Participant 10 explained how
her experience of hot flushes makes it “hard to concentrate” in meetings and affects her
“performance” at work. However, Participant 5 described a new “drive” within her to focus on
her work: “I feel like, oh God, I’m running out of time here I need to- I need to get the work
done, I need to get my work done. So that has been a big change. I'm very motivated.” In this
way, she found that her experience of menopause reflected positively on her work as an artist.
Positive changes in self-concept; a time for self-compassion and self-acceptance
“I look at it as a positive thing, not as a negative.” (P8)
Although the majority of participants acknowledged the negative impacts of their
menopause experience, a smaller number of participants demonstrated that their experience of
menopause has had a positive impact on their self-concept, including things like a more positive
self-image and body-image, greater self-esteem, and a newfound sense of resilience, self-
acceptance and self-compassion.
This theme was captured primarily from Participant 5 and Participant 8’s experience.
Participant 8 explained that she had a horrific experience with menstruation all her life which
resulted in many undesirable symptoms alongside low moods and low self-esteem. For her,
menopause has been an ultimately positive experience: “But all of that went. It seemed like it
just disappeared when I hit menopause.” She described feeling “happier in herself” upon
reaching menopause and explained that there is a new “lightness” about her. It should also be
noted that this participant did not make an association between menopause and age: “I don’t look
at it as getting old because I'm just me. I’m not different.”
Participant 5 experienced positive changes in her body-image. She describes that she now
accepts her curly hair which she hated for most of her life and doesn’t feel the need to wear
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 30
makeup anymore which she used to “mask” herself with: “That's something that's changed for
me. And I think it is menopause that has done that for me. I’ve become more accepting of
myself.” She does not deny that she experiences negative self-talk like the majority of women,
but she explains that she is able to “rationalize” it:
You do feel all those things. But I think my brain and myself have decided it's just a
whole load of nonsense, it really is. So yeah, I wouldn't say I have more confidence. But I
don't have less. I think I'm more accepting of this is the way I am now.
She described this self-acceptance as a new “resilience” that she hadn’t experienced before. With
this self-acceptance came a co-existing sense of “self-compassion,” as described by Participant 5.
She spoke of how this transition has made her “more protective of (her) tenderness” and
“gentler” on herself. She explained what she meant by this:
I'm saying no a lot more. Yeah, my self-care has risen and my behaviour has changed.
There are days when I go; I’m not putting on makeup today. I'm not going out. I'm not
doing it. I’m taking care of me. I'm watching a movie. I'm going out with my daughter.
Me first. Yeah, because you can feel kind of ‘bleh’, you feel old, you feel whatever. So
yeah, my behaviour has changed, but it's been in a more positive way.
She talks of how she views menopause as a positive opportunity for “self-reflection” and
“claiming back” her “me time,” particularly since her children are older now:
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 31
Up to this we've been so busy. You're busy, busy, busy work, work, work. And this is kind
of a slap in the face, where you have to stop and slow down and think. And yeah, you
start realizing how valuable your family is and how valuable you are, and you’ve worked
so hard all your life now you need to give yourself a break. Don’t be so hard on yourself.
Thus, it can be seen that for some women, this experience has positively impacted their
self-concept and has proven to be quite a positive experience.
Discussion
The aim of this research was to explore Irish women’s experience of the menopausal
transition and to understand if and how their experience has impacted their self-concept. Upon
analysing the interview transcripts, four key themes were constructed from the data: (i) Loss of
identity, womanhood and control, (ii) Negative self-appraisal, (iii) Impact of menopause and
self-concept on quality of life, (iv) Positive changes in self-concept; a time for self-compassion
and self-acceptance.
In the current study, most women described a predominantly negative self-concept during
this phase. However, some women experienced positive changes in their self-concept, while
others described themselves as not being very impacted by the transition at all. For those whose
self-concept was negatively impacted, they described large changes in their self-image and
identity such as having lost themselves, their fertility, their womanhood, and their control.
Similarly, they provide negative descriptions of themselves and their bodies, which subsequently
impacted their self-esteem and confidence in various contexts, resulting in shame and
embarrassment. Family, work, and relationships were mostly impacted by these changes which
often led to guilt, as well as women’s quality of life being compromised due to isolating
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 32
themselves and some using alcohol as a coping mechanism. A few participants were also
impacted by depression and some experienced suicidal thoughts during this time. On the other
end of the spectrum, some participants were not impacted by the aforementioned issues, and
some even experienced a positive change in their self-concept. For these women, greater moods
and self-esteem came with the freedom from troublesome menstruation, while others used their
new-found resilience to strive for more self-acceptance and self-compassion during a time which
they deemed to be about focusing more on themselves.
Consistent with Beyene (1986) and Deeks’ (2003) ideas that menopause is not solely a
hormonal event and the context of a woman’s life is crucial in understanding one’s experience of
menopause, it is clear that various psychosocial events adjacent to menopause had an impact on
participants’ self-concept in this study. For example, the impact of loss of fertility on a
participant who had previous difficulties having children, was quite different to that of the
improved mood and self-esteem for a participant who experienced great difficulty with
menstruation all of her life. It appears that no two women experience menopause in the same
way.
Although self-concept in menopausal women has been investigated by very few
researchers, some aspects of the themes identified can be seen in previous literature relating to
women’s experience of menopause. For example, most participants mentioned the impact that
menopause and self-concept had on their relationships, family and work, which has previously
been reported (Parish et al., 2019; Hvas & Gannik, 2008). Similarly, it could be seen within the
data that attitudes towards menopause varied for women at different stages of the transition,
which is consistent with previous literature (Brown et al., 2017; Busch et al., 2003). Overlapping
ideas could be seen between the current findings and the themes within the systematic review
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 33
conducted by Hoga et al. (2015); particularly the concept that women perceive menopause as a
time of gains and losses, that the physical and emotional changes of menopause strongly affect
them, and that resilience and coping strategies are improved to enhance wellbeing. With regards
to gains and losses, it appeared within the current study that very few women acknowledged both
within their experience; most women were concerned primarily with either the gains or the losses
associated with menopause. It is also clear from some of the heart-breaking descriptions from
participants that the changes of menopause strongly affected them, to the point of suicidal
ideation for some. They were also strongly affected by their body-image, which most women in
the current study described with negative terms, which is consistent with similar studies (e.g.
Deeks & McCabe, 2001; Walter, 2000). Resilience within the current study was used in a
positive way, however, the coping mechanisms mentioned (e.g. isolation and using alcohol) were
maladaptive, and therefore not used to enhance wellbeing.
Some of the discourses highlighted in Hvas and Gannik’s (2008) study could also be seen
in the way women spoke about their experience. The biomedical and forever young discourse
could be seen in the way many participants primarily associated menopause with physiological
symptoms, ageing and decline throughout the course of the interviews. All ten participants
mentioned doctors, HRT and age at some point during the interviews, either from a positive or
negative viewpoint. The alternative and existential discourse was also observed in some
participants, who were more likely to describe menopause as a natural and positive process full
of opportunity for self-reflection and personal growth.
By contrast to Carolan’s (2000) findings which highlighted that the rural Irish women in
their sample had a positive experience of menopause, the participants in this study experienced a
loss of identity, womanhood and control, negative self-appraisal and negative impacts on their
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 34
quality of life in addition to some positive changes in their self-concept. Little inferences can be
made due to the lag between the two studies and the fact that their sample only included rural
Irish women; however, the current findings greatly contribute to the limited research on the
experience of menopause within Irish populations.
Findings which have suggested that a positive self-concept predicts greater life
satisfaction, fewer physiological symptoms and less psychological distress during menopause
(Castiglione et al., 2015; Martínez et al., 2012; Shu et al., 2007) are reflected within the current
study, as those who described a more positive self-concept talked about fewer symptoms and less
distress during their experience of menopause. Consistent with Svenson’s (2005) findings, the
physical and emotional disruption of menopause also affected participants’ sense of self and self-
identity within the current study. The current study builds on this, and significantly adds to the
limited research surrounding women’s self-concept during menopause, particularly from a
qualitative approach.
It has been suggested that changes in self-concept during adolescence reflect a u-shaped
curve, with gradual increases in stability over time due to their sense of self becoming more
integrated and consistent (Harter, 1988). These self-perceptions have been found to be influenced
by biological (e.g. puberty) and cognitive factors, school transitions, etc. (Eccles et al., 1993). If
it is known that self-concept develops and changes throughout the lifespan (O’Brien, 1980;
Pasquali, 1999), there is no excuse for the lack of research into this construct during one of the
biggest biopsychological transitions in a woman’s life. Self-concept clarity has been related to
subjective well-being (Campbell et al., 1996; De Cremer & Sedikides, 2005; Lavallee &
Campbell, 1995; Slotter et al., 2010) and depression (Avis & McKinlay, 1991; Kaufert et al.,
1992), while positive self-concept dimensions have been associated with greater life satisfaction
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 35
(Martínez et al., 2012), fewer psychological and physiological menopause symptoms (Shu et al.,
2007) and better adaptation to menopausal changes (Castiglione et al., 2015). Therefore, as noted
by Guérin et al. (2017), more focus on women’s self-concept may allow for a healthier
psychological adjustment during this period.
Strengths and Limitations
One major strength of this study was the population that was investigated, as Carolan
(2000) had outlined the lack of research surrounding Irish women’s experience of menopause.
The research also addressed an apparent gap within the literature; the investigation of women’s
self-concept during menopause using a qualitative approach. As qualitative studies look at
individual experiences, the current study adds to the qualitative evidence, which provides a very
different insight to quantitative research. The qualitative methodology employed allowed for an
exploratory design and the use of thematic analysis in particular allowed for a non-rigid analysis
and interpretation of data. The use of one-to-one interviews were also beneficial as some of the
content that was discussed was quite emotional; these insights into women’s experience may not
have been obtained from the likes of focus groups.
However, some limitations of the study warrant mention. It should be noted that the
sampling strategy may have led to some bias within the findings, as it can be assumed that those
who were recruited within the menopause support groups were seeking support due to difficulties
encountered with their experience. Although the data provide a rich insight to the experience of
menopause for these 10 women, generalization to the larger population of Irish women is
limited. Data was not collected on whether participants were in receipt of HRT, or had been
suffering with comorbid psychiatric disorders, which may have impacted women’s self-concept.
Although all women within the sample met the inclusion criteria of experiencing menopausal
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 36
symptoms in the previous two years, data was not collected on the exact stage of the transition
they were at, i.e. perimenopause or post-menopause, which may account for some variance
within the data.
Implications and Future Research
The current findings highlight that every woman’s experience of menopause is different.
Along with the physical and psychological symptoms, women may experience negative changes
in their self-concept during menopause, including concerns with their changing identity, negative
self-appraisals, low self-esteem, etc., which may in turn impact on their quality of life and other
aspects of their lives such as family, relationships and work. By contrast, some women can
experience positive changes in their self-concept, or will not be impacted by the menopausal
transition at all.
These findings should inform health care practitioners in order to maximize support for
women during this developmental transition. Health care needs to be personal and tailored to the
individual needs, preferences and expectations of women, with due regard to social and cultural
contexts. Coping strategies should be developed to enable women who are struggling to manage
the psychological impact of menopause. The psychological implications of the menopausal
transition should not be neglected, as it is clear that menopause is experienced differently by
every woman.
The current study is a good foundation for which further research is needed. Such studies
might explore self-concept between distinct stages of the menopause transition; for example,
investigate self-concept within premenopausal, perimenopausal and postmenopausal women, to
see which groups may be most at risk of these negative implications. Some longitudinal research
might be necessary to investigate the direction of the relationship; the experience of menopause
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 37
may impact self-concept, but pre-existing self-concept might also impact psychological
adjustment during menopause.
In conclusion, the current study contributes to both the limited literature surrounding
Irish women’s experience of menopause, and the lack of studies which have investigated self-
concept in menopausal women, particularly using qualitative methodology. Four key themes
captured the essence of the data: (i) Loss of identity, womanhood and control, (ii) Negative self-
appraisal, (iii) Impact of menopause and self-concept on quality of life, (iv) Positive changes in
self-concept; a time for self-compassion and self-acceptance. These findings highlight the
changes in self-concept experienced by women during menopause and should be considered by
health care practitioners in order to maximize psychological outcomes for women during this
developmental transition.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 38
References
Adamson, L., Hartman, S. G., & Lyxell, B. (1999). Adolescent identity - a qualitative approach:
Self‐concept, existential questions and adult contacts. Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology, 40(1), 21-31.
Alderson, B. W. (1991). An overview of emotional issues faced by women over 50. In meeting of
the Association for Women in Psychology. Hartford, CT.
Avis, N. E., & Crawford, S. (2008). Cultural differences in symptoms and attitudes toward
menopause. Menopause Management, 17(3), 8.
Avis, N. E., & McKinlay, S. M. (1991). A longitudinal analysis of women's attitudes toward the
menopause: Results from the Massachusetts Women's Health Study. Maturitas, 13(1), 65-
79.
Avis, N. E., Brambilla, D., McKinlay, S. M., & Vass, K. (1994). A longitudinal analysis of the
association between menopause and depression: Results from the Massachusetts women's
health study. Annals of Epidemiology, 4(3), 214-220.
Avis, N. E., Crawford, S. L., & McKinlay, S. M. (1997). Psychosocial, behavioral, and health
factors related to menopause symptomatology. Women's Health, 3(2), 103-120.
Ayers, B., Forshaw, M., & Hunter, M. S. (2010). The impact of attitudes towards the menopause
on women's symptom experience: a systematic review. Maturitas, 65(1), 28-36.
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The
model of selective optimization with compensation. Successful aging: Perspectives from
the Behavioral Sciences, 1(1), 1-34.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Epstein, N., & Brown, G. (1990). Beck self-concept test. Psychological
Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2(2), 191.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 39
Beutel, M. E., Glaesmer, H., Decker, O., Fischbeck, S., & Brähler, E. (2009). Life satisfaction,
distress, and resiliency across the life span of women. Menopause, 16(6), 1132-1138.
Beyene, Y. (1986). Cultural significance and physiological manifestations of menopause: A
biocultural analysis. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 10(1), 47-71.
Bloch, A. (2002). Self-awareness during the menopause. Maturitas, 41(1), 61–68.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in
Psychology, 3(2), 77-101.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2019). To saturate or not to saturate? Questioning data saturation as a
useful concept for thematic analysis and sample-size rationales. Qualitative Research in
Sport, Exercise and Health, 1-16.
Brown, L., Brown, V., Judd, F., & Bryant, C. (2017). It’s not as bad as you think: menopausal
representations are more positive in postmenopausal women. Journal of Psychosomatic
Obstetrics & Gynecology, 1–8.
Burr, V. (2003). Social constructionism. East Sussex, UK: Routledge.
Busch, H., Barth-Olofsson, A. S., Rosenhagen, S., & Collins, A. (2003). Menopausal transition
and psychological development. Menopause, 10(2), 179-187.
Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L. F., & Lehman, D. R.
(1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and cultural
boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 141-156.
Carolan, M. (2000). Menopause: Irish women’s voices. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and
Neonatal Nursing, 29, 397-404.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 40
Cash, T. F. (1990). The psychology of physical appearance: Aesthetics, attributes, and images. In
T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance, and change (pp.
51-79). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Cash, T. F. (1997). The body image workbook: An 8-step program for learning to like your looks.
Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Cash, T. F., & Fleming, E. C. (2002). The impact of body image experiences: development of the
body image quality of life inventory. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31(4),
455-460.
Castiglione, C., Licciardello, O., & Rampullo, A. (2015). Possible future changes in menopausal
women. Life Span and Disability, 18(2), 189-205.
Chedraui, P., Pérez-López, F. R., Mendoza, M., Leimberg, M. L., Martínez, M. A., Vallarino, V.,
& Hidalgo, L. (2010). Assessment of self-esteem in mid-aged women. Maturitas, 66(1),
77-82.
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and mortality: a quantitative
review of prospective observational studies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70(7), 741-756.
Chrisler, J. C., & Ghiz, L. (1993). Body Image Issues of Older Women. Women & Therapy, 14(1-
2), 67–75.
De Cremer, D., & Sedikides, C. (2005). Self-uncertainty and responsiveness to procedural
justice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 157-173.
Deeks, A. A. (2003). Psychological aspects of menopause management. Best Practice &
Research: Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 17(1), 17-31.
Deeks, A. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2001). Menopausal stage and age and perceptions of body
image. Psychology & Health, 16(3), 367–379.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 41
Dennerstein, L. (1996). Well-being, symptoms and the menopausal transition. Maturitas, 23(2),
147–157.
Dennerstein, L., Smith, A. M., & Morse, C. (1994). Psychological well-being, mid-life and the
menopause. Maturitas, 20(1), 1–11.
Diener, E., & Diener, M. (2009). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction and self-esteem. In
Culture and Well-being (pp. 71-91). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer
Dolińska-Zygmunt, G., & Włodarczyk, M. (2012). Woman’s image in the menopausal period.
Polish Journal of Applied Psychology, 10(1), 65-74.
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Miller-Buchanan, C., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al.
(1993). Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-enviornment fit on young
adolescent’s experiences in schools and families. American Psychologist, 48, 90-101.
Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: a challenge for biomedicine. Science,
196(4286), 129-136.
Eynon, M. J., O’Donnell, C., & Williams, L. (2018). Gaining qualitative insight into the
subjective experiences of adherers to an exercise referral scheme: A thematic analysis.
Journal of Health Psychology, 23(11), 1476-1487.
Fitts, W. H., & Roid, G. H. (1964). Tennessee self concept scale. Nashville, TN: Counselor
Recordings and Tests.
Freeman, E. W., & Sherif, K. (2007). Prevalence of hot flushes and night sweats around the
world: a systematic review. Climacteric, 10(3), 197–214.
Gannon, L., & Ekstrom, B. (1993). Attitudes toward menopause: The influence of sociocultural
paradigms. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17(3), 275-288.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 42
George, S. A. (2002). The menopause experience: a woman's perspective. Journal of Obstetric,
Gynecologic, & Neonatal Nursing, 31(1), 77-85.
Greenberg, J. (2008). Understanding the vital human quest for self-esteem. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 3(1), 48-55.
Greenblatt, R. B., & Bruneteau, D. W. (1974). Menopausal headaches - psychogenic or
metabolic? Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 22(4), 186-190.
Greendale, G. A., Lee, N. P., & Arriola, E. R. (1999). The menopause. The Lancet, 353(9152),
571-580.
Guérin, E., Goldfield, G., & Prud’homme, D. (2017). Trajectories of mood and stress and
relationships with protective factors during the transition to menopause: Results using
latent class growth modeling in a Canadian cohort. Archives of Women's Mental Health,
20(6), 733-745.
Guest, G., Bunce, A., & Johnson, L. (2006). How many interviews are enough? An experiment
with data saturation and variability. Field methods, 18(1), 59-82.
Harrington, R., & Loffredo, D. A. (2001). The relationship between life satisfaction, self-
consciousness, and the Myers-Briggs type inventory dimensions. The Journal of
Psychology, 135(4), 439-450.
Harter, S. (1988). Causes, correlates, and the functional role of global self worth: A life-span
perspective. In J. Kolligian & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Perceptions of competence and
incompetence across the life-span (pp. 67-97). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Hashemipoor, F., Jafari, F., & Zabihi, R. (2019). Maladaptive schemas and psychological well-
being in premenopausal and postmenopausal women. Przeglad menopauzalny,
Menopause review, 18(1), 33.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 43
Heidari, M., Ghodusi, M., & Rafiei, H. (2017). Sexual Self-concept and its Relationship to
Depression, Stress and Anxiety in Postmenopausal Women. Journal of Menopausal
Medicine, 23(1), 42.
Hoga, L., Rodolpho, J., Gonçalves, B., & Quirino, B. (2015). Women's experience of
menopause: a systematic review of qualitative evidence. JBI Database of Systematic
Reviews and Implementation Reports, 13(8), 250-337.
Hunter, M., & Rendall, M. (2007). Bio-psycho-socio-cultural perspectives on menopause. Best
Practice & Research: Clinical Obstetrics & Gynaecology, 21(2), 261-274.
Hvas, L., & Gannik, D. E. (2008). Discourses on menopause - Part II: How do women talk about
menopause? Health: An Interdisciplinary Journal for the Social Study of Health, Illness
and Medicine, 12(2), 177–192.
Kahwati, L. C., Haigler, L., & Rideout, S. (2005). What is the best way to diagnose menopause?
Journal of Family Practice, 54(11), 1000-1002.
Kaufert, P. A., Gilbert, P., & Tate, R. (1992). The Manitoba Project: a re-examination of the link
between menopause and depression. Maturitas, 14(2), 143-155.
Kaufert, P. L. (1990). Methodological issue in menopause research. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, 592, 114-122.
Khademi, S., & Cooke, M. S. (2003). Comparing the attitudes of urban and rural Iranian women
towards menopause. Maturitas, 46(2), 113-122.
Kumari, M., Stafford, M., & Marmot, M. (2005). The menopausal transition was associated in a
prospective study with decreased health functioning in women who report menopausal
symptoms. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 58(7), 719–727.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 44
Lavallee, L. F., & Campbell, J. D. (1995). Impact of personal goals on self-regulation processes
elicited by daily negative events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 341-
352.
Liao, K. L. M., Wood, N., & Conway, G. S. (2000). Premature menopause and psychological
well-being. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology, 21(3), 167–174.
Lock, M. (1994). Menopause in cultural context. Experimental Gerontology, 29(3-4), 307-317.
Martin, M. C., Block, J. E., Sanchez, S. D., Arnaud, C. D., & Beyene, Y. (1993). Menopause
without symptoms: the endocrinology of menopause among rural Mayan Indians.
American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 168(6), 1839-1845.
Martínez, M. D., González-Arratia, N. I., Oudhof van Barneveld, J., Domínguez-Espinosa, A. D.
C., & Olivos-Rubio, M. (2012). Psychosocial Predictors of Satisfaction with Life during
Perimenopause and Postmenopause. Aquichan, 12(3), 298-307.
Marvan, M. L., Castillo-López, R. L., & Arroyo, L. (2013). Mexican beliefs and attitudes toward
menopause and menopausal-related symptoms. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics &
Gynecology, 34(1), 39-45.
McKinlay, S. M., Brambilla, D. J., & Posner, J. G. (1992). The normal menopause transition.
Maturitas, 14(2), 103-115.
McWhinney, I. R. (1997). A textbook of family medicine. New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Mohanty, N., & Sahoo, P. (2015). Self-concept and well-being of young adults: An empirical
study. Indian Journal of Health & Wellbeing, 6(1), 8–12.
O'Brien, J. (1980). Body image: mirror, mirror, why me? Nursing mirror, 150(17), 36.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 45
Parish, S. J., Faubion, S. S., Weinberg, M., Bernick, B., & Mirkin, S. (2019). The MATE survey:
men's perceptions and attitudes towards menopause and their role in partners’
menopausal transition. Menopause, 26(10), 1110-1116.
Parlee, M. B. (1990). Integrating biological and social scientific research on menopause. In M.
Flint, F. Kronenberg, & W. Utian (Eds.), Multidisciplinary perspectives on menopause:
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (vol. 592, pp. 378-389). New York, NY: The
New York Academy of Sciences.
Pasquali, E. A. (1999). The Impact of Premature Menopause on Women’s Experience of Self.
Journal of Holistic Nursing, 17(4), 346–364.
Pérez-López, F. R., Chedraui, P., Gilbert, J. J., & Pérez-Roncero, G. (2009). Cardiovascular risk
in menopausal women and prevalent related co-morbid conditions: facing the post-
Women's Health Initiative era. Fertility and Sterility, 92(4), 1171-1186.
Pinquart, M., & Sorensen, S. (2001). Gender Differences in Self-Concept and Psychological
Well-Being in Old Age: A Meta-Analysis. The Journals of Gerontology Series B:
Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 56(4), 195–213.
Pruzinsky, T., & Cash, T. F. (1990). Integrative themes in body image development, deviance,
and change. In T. F. Cash, & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance,
and change (pp. 337-349). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Richters, J. M. A. (1997). Menopause in different cultures. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics
& Gynecology, 18(2), 73-80.
Ritchie, T. D., Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Arndt, J., & Gidron, Y. (2011). Self-concept clarity
mediates the relation between stress and subjective well-being. Self and Identity, 10, 493-
508.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 46
Robinson, G. (1996). Cross-cultural perspectives on menopause. The Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 184(8), 453-458.
Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships: As
developed in the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A Study of a
Science. Formulations of the Person and the Social Context (Vol. 3, pp. 184-256). New
York, NY: McGraw Hill.
Rostosky, S. S., & Travis, C. B. (1996). Menopause research and the dominance of the
biomedical model 1984–1994. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20(2), 285-312.
Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Stanton, G. C. (1976). Self-concept: Validation of construct
interpretations. Review of Educational Research, 46(3), 407-441.
Shepard, M. K. (1990). Discussion: Part II. In M. Flint, F. Kronenberg, & W. Utian (Eds.),
Multidisciplinary perspectives on menopause: Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences (vol. 592, pp. 185-192). New York, NY: The New York Academy of Sciences.
Shu, B. C., Luh, W. M., Li, S. M., & Lu, S. Y. (2007). Self-concept and menopause among mid-
life women: A survey in southern Taiwan. Maturitas, 57(2), 132–138.
Sievert, L. L., & Espinosa-Hernandez, G. (2003). Attitudes toward menopause in relation to
symptom experience in Puebla, Mexico. Women & Health, 38(2), 93-106.
Slotter, E. B., Gardner, W. L., Finkel, E. J. (2010). Who am I without you? The influence of
romantic breakup on the self-concept. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36,
147-160.
Sowislo, J. F., & Orth, U. (2013). Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-
analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 213.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 47
Speroff, L. (2002). The perimenopause: definitions, demography, and physiology. Obstetrics and
Gynecology Clinics of North America, 29(3), 397-410.
Stewart, D. E. (2003). Menopause in highland Guatemala Mayan women. Maturitas, 44, 293-
297.
Stuenkel, C. A., Davis, S. R., Gompel, A., Lumsden, M. A., Murad, M. H., Pinkerton, J. V., &
Santen, R. J. (2015). Treatment of Symptoms of the Menopause: An Endocrine Society
Clinical Practice Guideline. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism,
100(11), 3975–4011.
Svenson, E. E. (2005). The woman’s experience of menopause: Its effect on her sense of self and
her marital/partnered relationship (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest.
https://search.proquest.com/openview/b68152447f8916de6d617b5c2768d259/1?cbl=187
50&diss=y&pq-origsite=gscholar
Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting beauty:
Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Treloar, A. E. (1974). Menarche, menopause, and intervening fecundability. Human Biology,
46(1), 89-107.
Treloar, A. E. (1981). Menstrual cyclicity and the pre-menopause. Maturitas, 3(3-4), 249-264.
von Keyserlingk, L., Becker, M., & Jansen, M. (2019). Academic self-concept during the
transition to upper secondary school. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 56, 152-
160.
Walter, C. A. (2000). The psychosocial meaning of menopause: women's experiences. Journal of
Women & Aging, 12(3-4), 117-131.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 48
Whitbourne, S. K. (1985). The psychological construction of the life span. In J. E. Birren, & K.
W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of ageing (2
nd
ed., pp. 594-618). New
York, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Williams, R. E., Kalilani, L., DiBenedetti, D. B., Zhou, X., Fehnel, S. E., & Clark, R. V. (2007).
Healthcare seeking and treatment for menopausal symptoms in the United States.
Maturitas, 58(4), 348-358.
Woods, N. F., & Mitchell, E. S. (1997). Pathways to depressed mood for midlife women:
observations from the Seattle Midlife Women's Health Study. Research in Nursing &
Health, 20(2), 119-129.
Woods, N. F., & Mitchell, E. S. (2005). Symptoms during the perimenopause: prevalence,
severity, trajectory, and significance in women’s lives. The American Journal of
Medicine, 118(12), 14–24.
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 49
Appendices
Appendix A
Interview Schedule
Research Question: How do women describe their self-concept during their experience of
natural menopause?
(The aim of this interview is to understand your ‘self-concept’ at this stage of your life. This
includes your self-image (how you view yourself/identity), self-esteem (the extent to which you
value yourself), perhaps body image, and how these perceptions may or may not impact your
experience of menopause.)
A. Introductory questions - Experiences
1. How has your experience of menopause been so far?
2. Is this experience different to what you anticipated or had previously learned?
B. Self-image/identity
1. How do you view yourself in this new stage of your life?
2. Is this view different or the same as before you knew you were experiencing menopause?
3. In what way do you think others view you, knowing that you have reached menopause?
(friends, family, partner, acquaintances)
4. How do you view other women experiencing menopause?
C. Self-esteem
1. How do you feel about the image of yourself that you described? Are you happy with it?
2. Is this feeling different or the same as before you knew you were experiencing
menopause?
D. Impact of self-concept
1. Do these feelings/concepts of yourself impact your behaviour?
2. Do these feelings/concepts impact any other aspects of your life? (e.g. work,
relationships, children…)
E. Closing questions
1. Is there anything that has influenced your self-concept?
2. Is there anything that could change your self-concept?
3. As a woman living in Ireland, how do you feel women experiencing the menopausal
transition are viewed in our society?
4. Has there been any particular cultural influences that have affected you?
5. Is there anything else that you would like to talk about with regards to the menopausal
transition that perhaps I have not mentioned?
PROBES:
Could you tell me a little more about that?
Could you tell me about a particular time that you experienced this/felt this way?
WOMEN’S SELF-CONCEPT DURING MENOPAUSE 50
Appendix B
Participant Information Sheet
Study Title: A Qualitative Study of Women’s Self-Concept During Menopause
What is the purpose of the study?
The aim of this study is to explore women’s self-concept during their menopausal transition, in
an effort to identify potential patterns or themes shared by women/differentiating women during
this phase of life. This includes questions about self-image, identity, self-esteem, etc., which is
essentially how you think and feel about yourself at this stage of your life. There is a lack of
research in this area, particularly in Ireland, which is why this is the topic of interest. Ultimately,
this will lead to a greater understanding of women’s experiences, and a greater opportunity for
future interventions that could potentially improve women’s self-concept during the menopausal
transition.
Can you take part in this study?
For the purpose
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uqrp20
Qualitative Research in Psychology
ISSN: 1478-0887 (Print) 1478-0895 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uqrp20
Using thematic analysis in psychology
Virginia Braun & Victoria Clarke
To cite this article: Virginia Braun & Victoria Clarke (2006) Using thematic analysis in psychology,
Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3:2, 77-101
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Published online: 21 Jul 2008.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 174409
Citing articles: 21805 View citing articles
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uqrp20
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uqrp20
https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCode=uqrp20&show=instructions
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCode=uqrp20&show=instructions
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa#tabModule
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa#tabModule
Using thematic analysis in
psychology
Virginia Braun1 and Victoria Clarke2
1University of Auckland and 2University of the West of England
Thematic analysis is a poorly demarcated, rarely acknowledged, yet widely
used qualitative analytic method within psychology. In this paper, we
argue that it offers an accessible and theoretically flexible approach to
analysing qualitative data. We outline what thematic analysis is, locating it
in relation to other qualitative analytic methods that search for themes or
patterns, and in relation to different epistemological and ontological
positions. We then provide clear guidelines to those wanting to start
thematic analysis, or conduct it in a more deliberate and rigorous way, and
consider potential pitfalls in conducting thematic analysis. Finally, we
outline the disadvantages and advantages of thematic analysis. We
conclude by advocating thematic analysis as a useful and flexible method
for qualitative research in and beyond psychology. Qualitative Research in
Psychology 2006; 3: 77�/101
Key words: epistemology; flexibility; patterns; qualitative psychology;
thematic analysis
Thematic analysis is a poorly demarcated
and rarely acknowledged, yet widely used
qualitative analytic method (Boyatzis,
1998; Roulston, 2001) within and beyond
psychology. In this paper, we aim to fill
what we, as researchers and teachers in
qualitative psychology, have experienced
as a current gap �/ the absence of a paper
which adequately outlines the theory, ap-
plication and evaluation of thematic ana-
lysis, and one which does so in a way
accessible to students and those not parti-
cularly familiar with qualitative research.1
That is, we aim to write a paper that will
be useful as both a teaching and research
tool in qualitative psychology. Therefore,
in this paper we discuss theory and
method for thematic analysis, and clarify
Correspondence: Virginia Braun, Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland,
New Zealand.
E-mail: v.braun@auckland.ac.nz
# 2006 Edward Arnold (Publishers) Ltd 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Qualitative Research in Psychology 2006; 3: 77�/101www.QualResearchPsych.com
the similarities and differences between
different approaches that share features in
common with a thematic approach.
Qualitative approaches are incredibly
diverse, complex and nuanced (Holloway
and Todres, 2003), and thematic analysis
should be seen as a foundational method
for qualitative analysis. It is the first
qualitative method of analysis that re-
searchers should learn, as it provides core
skills that will be useful for conducting
many other forms of qualitative analysis.
Indeed, Holloway and Todres (2003: 347)
identify ‘thematizing meanings’ as one of a
few shared generic skills across qualitative
analysis.2 For this reason, Boyatzis (1998)
characterizes it, not as a specific method,
but as a tool to use across different meth-
ods. Similarly, Ryan and Bernard (2000)
locate thematic coding as a process per-
formed within ‘major’ analytic traditions
(such as grounded theory), rather than a
specific approach in its own right. We
argue thematic analysis should be consid-
ered a method in its own right.
One of the benefits of thematic analysis is
its flexibility. Qualitative analytic methods
can be roughly divided into two camps.
Within the first, there are those tied to, or
stemming from, a particular theoretical or
epistemological position. For some of these
�/ such as conversation analysis (CA; eg,
Hutchby and Wooffitt, 1998) and interpre-
tative phenomenological analysis (IPA; eg,
Smith and Osborn, 2003) �/ there is (as yet)
relatively limited variability in how the
method is applied, within that framework.
In essence, one recipe guides analysis. For
others of these �/ such as grounded theory
(Glaser, 1992; Strauss and Corbin, 1998),
discourse analysis (DA; eg, Burman and
Parker, 1993; Potter and Wetherell, 1987;
Willig, 2003) or narrative analysis (Murray,
2003; Riessman, 1993) �/ there are different
manifestations of the method, from within
the broad theoretical framework. Second,
there are methods that are essentially in-
dependent of theory and epistemology, and
can be applied across a range of theoretical
and epistemological approaches. Although
often (implicitly) framed as a realist/experi-
ential method (Aronson, 1994; Roulston,
2001), thematic analysis is actually firmly
in the second camp, and is compatible with
both essentialist and constructionist para-
digms within psychology (we discuss this
later). Through its theoretical freedom, the-
matic analysis provides a flexible and use-
ful research tool, which can potentially
provide a rich and detailed, yet complex,
account of data.
Given the advantages of the flexibility of
thematic analysis, it is important that we are
clear that we are not trying to limit this
flexibility. However, an absence of clear and
concise guidelines around thematic analysis
means that the ‘anything goes’ critique of
qualitative research (Antaki et al ., 2002) may
well apply in some instances. With this
paper, we hope to strike a balance between
demarcating thematic analysis clearly �/ ie,
explaining what it is, and how to do it �/ and
ensuring flexibility in relation to how it is
used, so that it does not become limited and
constrained, and lose one of its key advan-
tages. Indeed, a clear demarcation of this
method will be useful to ensure that those
who use thematic analysis can make active
choices about the particular form of analysis
they are engaged in. Therefore, this paper
seeks to celebrate the flexibility of the
method and provide a vocabulary and
‘recipe’ for people to undertake thematic
analysis in a way that is theoretically and
methodologically sound.3 As we will show,
what is important is that as well as apply-
ing a method to data, researchers make
their (epistemological and other) assump-
78 V Braun and V Clarke
tions explicit (Holloway and Todres, 2003).
Qualitative psychologists need to be clear
about what they are doing and why, and to
include the often-omitted ‘how’ they did
their analysis in their reports (Attride-
Stirling, 2001).
In this paper we outline: what thematic
analysis is; a 6-phase guide to performing
thematic analysis; potential pitfalls to
avoid when doing thematic analysis; what
makes good thematic analysis; and advan-
tages and disadvantages of thematic analy-
sis. Throughout, we provide exam-
ples from the research literature, and our
own research. By providing examples, we
show the types of research questions and
topics that thematic analysis can be used to
study.
Before we begin, we need to define a few
of the terms used throughout the paper.
Data corpus refers to all data collected for
a particular research project, while data set
refers to all the data from the corpus that
are being used for a particular analysis.
There are two main ways of choosing the
data set (which approach you take depends
on whether you are coming to the data
with a specific question or not �/ see ‘A
number of decisions’ below). First, the data
set may consist of many, or all, individual
data items within your data corpus. So, for
example, in a project on female genital
cosmetic surgery, Virginia’s data corpus
consists of interviews with surgeons,
media items on the topic, and surgeon
websites. For any particular analysis, her
data set might just be the surgeon inter-
views, just the websites (Braun, 2005b), or
it might combine surgeon data with some
media data (eg, Braun, 2005a). Second, the
data set might be identified by a particular
analytic interest in some topic in the data,
and the data set then becomes all instances
in the corpus where that topic is referred.
So in Virginia’s example, if she was inter-
ested in how ‘sexual pleasure’ was talked
about, her data set would consist of all
instances across the entire data corpus that
had some relevance to sexual pleasure.
These two approaches might sometimes
be combined to produce the data set. Data
item is used to refer to each individual
piece of data collected, which together
make up the data set or corpus. A data
item in this instance would be an indivi-
dual surgeon interview, a television docu-
mentary, or one particular website. Finally,
data extract refers to an individual coded
chunk of data, which has been identified
within, and extracted from, a data item.
There will be many of these, taken from
throughout the entire data set, and only a
selection of these extracts will feature in
the final analysis.
What is thematic analysis?
Thematic analysis is a method for identify-
ing, analysing and reporting patterns
(themes) within data. It minimally orga-
nizes and describes your data set in (rich)
detail. However, frequently if goes further
than this, and interprets various aspects of
the research topic (Boyatzis, 1998). The
range of different possible thematic ana-
lyses will further be highlighted in relation
to a number of decisions regarding it as a
method (see below).
Thematic analysis is widely used, but
there is no clear agreement about what
thematic analysis is and how you go about
doing it (see Attride-Stirling, 2001; Boyat-
zis, 1998; Tuckett, 2005, for other exam-
ples). It can be seen as a very poorly
‘branded’ method, in that it does not appear
to exist as a ‘named’ analysis in the same
way that other methods do (eg, narrative
Using thematic analysis in psychology 79
analysis, grounded theory). In this sense, it
is often not explicitly claimed as the
method of analysis, when, in actuality, we
argue that a lot of analysis is essentially
thematic �/ but is either claimed as some-
thing else (such as DA, or even content
analysis (eg, Meehan et al ., 2000)) or not
identified as any particular method at all �/
for example, data were ‘subjected to quali-
tative analysis for commonly recurring
themes’ (Braun and Wilkinson, 2003: 30).
If we do not know how people went about
analysing their data, or what assumptions
informed their analysis, it is difficult to
evaluate their research, and to compare
and/or synthesize it with other studies on
that topic, and it can impede other research-
ers carrying out related projects in the
future (Attride-Stirling, 2001). For these
reasons alone, clarity on process and prac-
tice of method is vital. We hope that this
paper will lead to more clarity around
thematic analysis.
Relatedly, insufficient detail is often gi-
ven to reporting the process and detail of
analysis (Attride-Stirling, 2001). It is not
uncommon to read of themes ‘emerging’
from the data (although this issue is not
limited to thematic analysis). For example,
Singer and Hunter’s (1999: 67) thematic
discourse analysis of women’s experiences
of early menopause identified that ‘several
themes emerged’ during the analysis. Rubin
and Rubin (1995: 226) claim that analysis is
exciting because ‘you discover themes and
concepts embedded throughout your inter-
views’. An account of themes ‘emerging’ or
being ‘discovered’ is a passive account of
the process of analysis, and it denies the
active role the researcher always plays in
identifying patterns/themes, selecting
which are of interest, and reporting them
to the readers (Taylor and Ussher, 2001).4
The language of ‘themes emerging’:
can be misinterpreted to mean that themes ‘re-
side’ in the data, and if we just look hard enough
they will ‘emerge’ like Venus on the half shell.
If themes ‘reside’ anywhere, they reside in
our heads from our thinking about our data and
creating links as we understand them. (Ely et al .,
1997: 205�/6)
At this point, it is important to acknowledge
our own theoretical positions and values in
relation to qualitative research. We do not
subscribe to a naı̈ve realist view of qualita-
tive research, where the researcher can
simply ‘give voice’ (see Fine, 2002) to their
participants. As Fine (2002): 218) argues,
even a ‘giving voice’ approach ‘involves
carving out unacknowledged pieces of
narrative evidence that we select, edit,
and deploy to border our arguments’. How-
ever, nor do we think there is one ideal
theoretical framework for conducting quali-
tative research, or indeed one ideal method.
What is important is that the theoretical
framework and methods match what the
researcher wants to know, and that they
acknowledge these decisions, and recognize
them as decisions.
Thematic analysis differs from other ana-
lytic methods that seek to describe patterns
across qualitative data �/ such as ‘thematic’
DA, thematic decomposition analysis, IPA
and grounded theory.5 Both IPA and
grounded theory seek patterns in the data,
but are theoretically bounded. IPA is at-
tached to a phenomenological epistemology
(Smith et al ., 1999; Smith and Osborn,
2003), which gives experience primacy
(Holloway and Todres, 2003), and is about
understanding people’s everyday experi-
ence of reality, in great detail, in order to
gain an understanding of the phenomenon
in question (McLeod, 2001). To complicate
matters, grounded theory comes in different
versions (Charmaz, 2002). Regardless, the
goal of a grounded theory analysis is to
generate a plausible �/ and useful �/ theory
80 V Braun and V Clarke
of the phenomena that is grounded in the
data (McLeod, 2001). However, in our ex-
perience, grounded theory seems increas-
ingly to be used in a way that is essentially
grounded theory ‘lite’ �/ as a set of proce-
dures for coding data very much akin to
thematic analysis. Such analyses do not
appear to fully subscribe to the theoretical
commitments of a ‘full-fat’ grounded theory,
which requires analysis to be directed to-
wards theory development (Holloway and
Todres, 2003). We argue, therefore, that a
‘named and claimed’ thematic analysis
means researchers need not subscribe to
the implicit theoretical commitments of
grounded theory if they do not wish to
produce a fully worked-up grounded-theory
analysis.
The term ‘thematic DA’ is used to refer to
a wide range of pattern-type analysis of
data, ranging from thematic analysis within
a social constructionist epistemology (ie,
where patterns are identified as socially
produced, but no discursive analyse is
conducted), to forms of analysis very
much akin to the interpretative repertoire
form of DA (Clarke, 2005). Thematic decom-
position analysis (eg, Stenner, 1993; Ussher
and Mooney-Somers, 2000) is a specifically
named form of ‘thematic’ DA, which iden-
tifies patterns (themes, stories) within data,
and theorizes language as constitutive of
meaning and meaning as social.
These different methods share a search
for certain themes or patterns across an
(entire) data set, rather than within a data
item, such as an individual interview or
interviews from one person, as in the case of
biographical or case-study forms of analy-
sis, such as narrative analysis (eg, Murray,
2003; Riessman, 1993). In this sense, they
more or less overlap with thematic analysis.
As thematic analysis does not require the
detailed theoretical and technological
knowledge of approaches, such as grounded
theory and DA, it can offer a more accessible
form of analysis, particularly for those early
in a qualitative research career.
In contrast to IPA or grounded theory (and
other methods like narrative analysis DA or
CA), thematic analysis is not wedded to any
pre-existing theoretical framework, and
therefore it can be used within different
theoretical frameworks (although not all),
and can be used to do different things
within them. Thematic analysis can be an
essentialist or realist method, which reports
experiences, meanings and the reality of
participants, or it can be a constructionist
method, which examines the ways in which
events, realities, meanings, experiences and
so on are the effects of a range of discourses
operating within society. It can also be a
‘contextualist’ method, sitting between the
two poles of essentialism and construction-
ism, and characterized by theories, such as
critical realism (eg, Willig, 1999), which
acknowledge the ways individuals make
meaning of their experience, and, in turn,
the ways the broader social context im-
pinges on those meanings, while retaining
focus on the material and other limits of
‘reality’. Therefore, thematic analysis can be
a method that works both to reflect reality
and to unpick or unravel the surface of
‘reality’. However, it is important that the
theoretical position of a thematic analysis is
made clear, as this is all too often left
unspoken (and is then typically a realist
account). Any theoretical framework carries
with it a number of assumptions about the
nature of the data, what they represent in
terms of the ‘the world’, ‘reality’, and so
forth. A good thematic analysis will make
this transparent.
A number of decisions
Thematic analysis involves a number of
choices which are often not made explicit
Using thematic analysis in psychology 81
(or are certainly typically not discussed in
the method section of papers), but which
need explicitly to be considered and dis-
cussed. In practice, these questions should
be considered before analysis (and some-
times even collection) of the data begins,
and there needs to be an ongoing reflexive
dialogue on the part of the researcher or
researchers with regards to these issues,
throughout the analytic process. The
method section of Taylor and Ussher’s
(2001) thematic DA of S&M provides a
good example of research which presents
this process explicitly; the method section
of Braun and Wilkinson (2003) does not.
What counts as a theme?
A theme captures something important
about the data in relation to the research
question, and represents some level of
patterned response or meaning within the
data set. An important question to address
in terms of coding is: what counts as a
pattern/theme, or what ‘size’ does a theme
need to be? This is a question of prevalence,
in terms both of space within each data item
and of prevalence across the entire data set.
Ideally, there will be a number of instances
of the theme across the data set, but more
instances do not necessarily mean the
theme itself is more crucial. As this is
qualitative analysis, there is no hard-and-
fast answer to the question of what propor-
tion of your data set needs to display
evidence of the theme for it to be considered
a theme. It is not the case that if it was
present in 50% of one’s data items, it would
be a theme, but if it was present only in
47%, then it would not be a theme. Nor is it
the case that a theme is only something that
many data items give considerable attention
to, rather than a sentence or two. A theme
might be given considerable space in some
data items, and little or none in others, or it
might appear in relatively little of the data
set. So, researcher judgement is necessary to
determine what a theme is. Our initial
guidance around this is that you need to
retain some flexibility, and rigid rules really
do not work. (The question of prevalence is
revisited in relation to themes and sub-
themes, as the refinement of analysis (see
later) will often result in overall themes,
and sub-themes within those.)
Furthermore, the ‘keyness’ of a theme is
not necessarily dependent on quantifiable
measures �/ but rather on whether it cap-
tures something important in relation to
the overall research question. For exam-
ple, in Victoria’s research on representa-
tions of lesbians and gay parents on 26
talk shows (Clarke and Kitzinger, 2004),
she identified six ‘key’ themes. These six
themes were not necessarily the most pre-
valent themes across the data set �/ they
appeared in between two and 22 of the 26
talk shows �/ but together they captured an
important element of the way in which
lesbians and gay men ‘normalize’ their
families in talk show debates. In this in-
stance, her thematic analysis was driven by
this particular analytic question. How she
‘measured’ prevalence is relevant, as pre-
valence can be determined in a number of
different ways. Prevalence was counted at
the level of the data item (ie, did a theme
appear anywhere in each individual talk
show?). Alternatively, it could have been
counted in terms of the number of different
speakers who articulated the theme, across
the entire data set, or each individual
occurrence of the theme across the entire
data set (which raises complex questions
about where an ‘instance’ begins and ends
within an extended sequence of talk �/ see
Riessman, 1993). Because prevalence was
not crucial to the analysis presented, Vic-
toria chose the most straightforward form,
82 V Braun and V Clarke
but it is important to note there is no right or
wrong method for determining prevalence.
Part of the flexibility of thematic analysis
is that it allows you to determine themes
(and prevalence) in a number of ways. What
is important is that you are consistent in
how you do this within any particular
analysis.
There are various ‘conventions’ for repre-
senting prevalence in thematic (and other
qualitative) analysis that does not provide a
quantified measure (unlike much content
analysis, Wilkinson, 2000) �/ for instance:
‘the majority of participants’ (Meehan et al .,
2000: 372), ‘many participants’ (Taylor and
Ussher, 2001: 298), or ‘a number of
participants’ (Braun et al ., 2003: 249).
Such descriptors work rhetorically to
suggest a theme really existed in the data,
and to convince us they are reporting
truthfully about the data. But do they tell
us much? This is perhaps one area where
more debate is needed about how and why
we might represent the prevalence of
themes in the data, and, indeed, whether,
if, and why prevalence is particularly im-
portant.
A rich description of the data set, or a
detailed account of one particular aspect
It is important to determine the type of
analysis you want to do, and the claims
you want to make, in relation to your data
set. For instance, you might wish to provide
a rich thematic description of your entire
data set, so that the reader gets a sense of the
predominant or important themes. In this
case, the themes you identify, code, and
analyse would need to be an accurate reflec-
tion of the content of the entire data set. In
such an analysis, some depth and complex-
ity is necessarily lost (particularly if you are
writing a short dissertation or article with
strict word limits), but a rich overall de-
scription is maintained. This might be a
particularly useful method when you are
investigating an under-researched area, or
you are working with participants whose
views on the topic are not known.
An alternative use of thematic analysis is
to provide a more detailed and nuanced
account of one particular theme, or group of
themes, within the data. This might relate to
a specific question or area of interest within
the data (a semantic approach �/ see below),
or to a particular ‘latent’ theme (see below)
across the whole or majority of the data set.
An example of this would be Victoria’s talk
show paper, discussed previously (Clarke
and Kitzinger, 2004), which examined nor-
malization in lesbians’ and gay men’s ac-
counts of parenting.
Inductive versus theoretical thematic
analysis
Themes or patterns within data can be
identified in one of two primary ways in
thematic analysis: in an inductive or ‘bot-
tom up’ way (eg, Frith and Gleeson, 2004),
or in a theoretical or deductive or ‘top
down’ way (eg, Boyatzis, 1998; Hayes,
1997). An inductive approach means the
themes identified are strongly linked to the
data themselves (Patton, 1990) (as such, this
form of thematic analysis bears some simi-
larity to grounded theory). In this approach,
if the data have been collected specifically
for the research (eg, via interview or focus
group), the themes identified may bear little
relation to the specific questions that were
asked of the participants. They would also
not be driven by the researcher’s theoretical
interest in the area or topic. Inductive
analysis is therefore a process of coding
the data without trying to fit it into a pre-
existing coding frame, or the researcher’s
analytic preconceptions. In this sense, this
form of thematic analysis is data-driven.
Using thematic analysis in psychology 83
However, it is important to note, as we
discussed earlier, that researchers cannot
free themselves of their theoretical and
epistemological commitments, and data
are not coded in an epistemological va-
cuum.
In contrast, a ‘theoretical’ thematic analy-
sis would tend to be driven by the research-
er’s theoretical or analytic interest in the
area, and is thus more explicitly analyst-
driven. This form of thematic analysis tends
to provide less a rich description of the data
overall, and more a detailed analysis of
some aspect of the data. Additionally, the
choice between inductive and theoretical
maps onto how and why you are coding the
data. You can either code for a quite specific
research question (which maps onto the
more theoretical approach) or the specific
research question can evolve through the
coding process (which maps onto the in-
ductive approach).
For example, if a researcher was inter-
ested in talk about heterosex, and had
collected interview data, with an inductive
approach they would read and re-read the
data for any themes related to heterosex,
and code diversely, without paying atten-
tion to the themes that previous research on
the topic might have identified. For exam-
ple, the researcher would not look to the
influential research of Hollway (1989),
identifying discourses of heterosex, and
code just for male sexual drive, have/hold
or permissive discourse themes. In contrast,
with a theoretical approach, the researcher
may well be interested in the way permis-
siveness plays out across the data, and
focus on that particular feature in coding
the data. This would then result in a
number of themes around permissiveness,
which may include, speak to, or expand on
something approximating Hollway’s origi-
nal theme.
Semantic or latent themes
Another decision revolves around the ‘le-
vel’ at which themes are to be identified: at
a semantic or explicit level, or at a latent or
interpretative level (Boyatzis, 1998).6 A
thematic analysis typically focuses exclu-
sively or primarily on one level. With a
semantic approach, the themes are identi-
fied within the explicit or surface meanings
of the data, and the analyst is not looking for
anything beyond what a participant has
said or what has been written. Ideally, the
analytic process involves a progression from
description , where the data have simply
been organized to show patterns in seman-
tic content, and summarized, to interpreta-
tion , where there is an attempt to theorize
the significance of the patterns and their
broader meanings and implications (Patton,
1990), often in relation to previous literature
(for an excellent example of this, see Frith
and Gleeson, 2004).
In contrast, a thematic analysis at the
latent level goes beyond the semantic con-
tent of the data, and starts to identify or
examine the underlying ideas, assumptions,
and conceptualizations �/ and ideologies �/
that are theorized as shaping or informing
the semantic content of the data. If we
imagine our data three-dimensionally as
an uneven blob of jelly, the semantic
approach would seek to describe the surface
of the jelly, its form and meaning, while the
latent approach would seek to identify the
features that gave it that particular form and
meaning. Thus, for latent thematic analysis,
the development of the themes themselves
involves interpretative work, and the ana-
lysis that is produced is not just descrip-
tion, but is already theorized.
Analysis within this latter tradition tends
to come from a constructionist paradigm
(eg, Burr, 1995), and in this form, thematic
analysis overlaps with some forms of ‘DA’
84 V Braun and V Clarke
(which are sometimes specifically referred
to as ‘thematic DA’ (eg, Singer and Hunter,
1999; Taylor and Ussher, 2001)), where
broader assumptions, structures and/or
meanings are theorized as underpinning
what is actually articulated in the data.
Increasingly, a number of discourse analysts
are also revisiting psycho-analytic modes of
interpretation (eg, Hollway and Jefferson,
2000), and latent thematic analysis would
also be compatible with that framework.
Epistemology: essentialist/realist versus
constructionist thematic analysis
As we have argued, thematic analysis can be
conducted within both realist/essentialist
and constructionist paradigms, although
the outcome and focus will be different for
each. The question of epistemology is
usually determined when a research project
is being conceptualized, although episte-
mology may also raise its head again during
analysis, when the research focus may shift
to an interest in different aspects of the data.
The research epistemology guides what you
can say about your data, and informs how
you theorize meaning. For instance, with an
essentialist/realist approach, you can theo-
rize motivations, experience, and meaning
in a straightforward way, because a simple,
largely unidirectional relationship is as-
sumed between meaning and experience
and language (language reflects and enables
us to articulate meaning and experience)
(Potter and Wetherell, 1987; Widdicombe
and Wooffitt, 1995).
In contrast, from a constructionist per-
spective, meaning and experience are so-
cially produced and reproduced, rather
than inhering within individuals (Burr,
1995). Therefore, thematic analysis con-
ducted within a constructionist framework
cannot and does not seek to focus on
motivation or individual psychologies, but
instead seeks to theorize the sociocultural
contexts, and structural conditions, that
enable the individual accounts that are
provided. Thematic analysis that focuses
on ‘latent’ themes tends to be more con-
structionist, and it also tends to start to
overlap with thematic DA at this point.
However, not all ‘latent’ thematic analysis
is constructionist.
The many questions of qualitative research
It is worth briefly noting that qualitative
research involves a series of questions, and
there is a need to be clear about the relation-
ship between these different questions.
First, there is the overall research question
or questions that drive the project. A re-
search question might be very broad (and
exploratory), such as ‘how is lesbian and
gay parenting constructed?’ or ‘what are the
meanings of the vagina?’. Narrower research
questions might be ‘how and why is lesbian
and gay parenting normalized?’ (Clarke and
Kitzinger, 2004), or ‘what are the discourses
around vaginal size?’ (see Braun and Kit-
zinger, 2001). These narrow questions may
be part of a broader overarching research
question, and if so, the analyses they inform
would also provide answers to the overall
research question. Although all projects are
guided by research questions, these may
also be refined as a project progresses.
Second, if data from interviews or focus
groups have been collected, there are the
questions that participants have responded
to. Finally, there are the questions that
guide the coding and analysis of the data.
There is no necessary relationship between
these three, and indeed, it is often desirable
that there is a disjuncture between them.
Some of the worst examples of ‘thematic’
analysis we have read have simply used
the questions put to participants as the
‘themes’ identified in the ‘analysis’ �/
Using thematic analysis in psychology 85
although in such instances, no analysis has
really been done at all!
To sum up, thematic analysis involves the
searching across a data set �/ be that a
number of interviews or focus groups, or a
range of texts �/ to find repeated patterns of
meaning. The exact form and product of
thematic analysis varies, as indicated above,
and so it is important that the questions
outlined above are considered before and
during thematic analyses. Those appro-
aches which consider specific aspects, la-
tent themes and are constructionist tend to
often cluster together, while those that
consider meanings across the whole data
set, semantic themes, and are realist, often
cluster together. However, there are no hard-
and-fast rules in relation to this, and differ-
ent combinations are possible. What is
important is that the finished product con-
tains an account �/ not necessarily that
detailed �/ of what was done, and why. So
what does one actually do? We now provide
what is, we hope, a straightforward step-
by-step guide to conducting thematic ana-
lysis.
Doing thematic analysis: a step-by-step
guide
Some of the phases of thematic analysis are
similar to the phases of other qualitative
research, so these stages are not necessarily
all unique to thematic analysis. The process
starts when the analyst begins to notice,
and look for, patterns of meaning and
issues of potential interest in the data �/
this may be during data collection. The
endpoint is the reporting of the content
and meaning of patterns (themes) in the
data, where ‘themes are abstract (and often
fuzzy) constructs the investigators identify
[sic] before, during, and after analysis’
(Ryan and Bernard, 2000: 780). Analysis
involves a constant moving back and for-
ward between the entire data set, the coded
extracts of data that you are analysing, and
the analysis of the data that you are produ-
cing. Writing is an integral part of analysis,
not something that takes place at the end, as
it does with statistical analyses. Therefore,
writing should begin in phase one, with the
jotting down of ideas and potential coding
schemes, and continue right through the
entire coding/analysis process.
There are different positions regarding
when you should engage with the literature
relevant to your analysis �/ with some
arguing that early reading can narrow your
analytic field of vision, leading you to focus
on some aspects of the data at the expense
of other potentially crucial aspects. Others
argue that engagement with the literature
can enhance your analysis by sensitizing
you to more subtle features of the data
(Tuckett, 2005). Therefore, there is no one
right way to proceed with reading for the-
matic analysis, although a more inductive
approach would be enhanced by not enga-
ging with literature in the early stages of
analysis, whereas a theoretical approach
requires engagement with the literature
prior to analysis.
We provide an outline guide through the
six phases of analysis, and offer examples to
demonstrate the process.7 The different
phases are summarized in Table 1. It is
important to recognize that qualitative ana-
lysis guidelines are exactly that �/ they are
not rules, and, following the basic precepts,
will need to be applied flexibly to fit the
research questions and data (Patton, 1990).
Moreover, analysis is not a linear process of
simply moving from one phase to the next.
Instead, it is more recursive process, where
movement is back and forth as needed,
throughout the phases. It is also a process
86 V Braun and V Clarke
that develops over time (Ely et al ., 1997),
and should not be rushed.
Phase 1: familiarizing yourself with your
data
When you engage in analysis, you may have
collected the data yourself, or they may have
been given to you. If you collected them
through interactive means, you will come to
the analysis with some prior knowledge of
the data, and possibly some initial analytic
interests or thoughts. Regardless, it is vital
that you immerse yourself in the data to the
extent that you are familiar with the depth
and breadth of the content. Immersion
usually involves ‘repeated reading’ of the
data, and reading the data in an active way �/
searching for meanings, patterns and so on.
It is ideal to read through the entire data set
at least once before you begin your coding,
as ideas and identification of possible pat-
terns will be shaped as you read through.
Whether or not you are aiming for an
overall or detailed analysis, are searching
for latent or semantic themes, or are data- or
theoretically-driven will inform how the
reading proceeds. Regardless, it is impor-
tant to be familiar with all aspects of your
data. At this phase, one of the reasons why
qualitative research tends to use far smaller
samples than, for example, questionnaire
research will become apparent �/ the read-
ing and re-reading of data is time-consum-
ing. It is, therefore, tempting to skip over
this phase, or be selective. We would
strongly advise against this, as this phase
provides the bedrock for the rest of the
analysis.
During this phase, it is a good idea to start
taking notes or marking ideas for coding
that you will then go back to in subsequent
phases. Once you have done this, you are
ready to begin, the more formal coding
process. In essence, coding continues to be
developed and defined throughout the en-
tire analysis.
Transcription of verbal data
If you are working with verbal data, such as
interviews, television programmes or poli-
tical speeches, the data will need to be
transcribed into written form in order to
conduct a thematic analysis. The process of
transcription, while it may seen time-con-
suming, frustrating, and at times boring, can
be an excellent way to start familiarizing
yourself with the data (Riessman, 1993).
Further, some researchers even argue
it should be seen as ‘a key phase of
data analysis within interpretative qualita-
tive methodology’ (Bird, 2005: 227), and
recognized as an interpretative act, where
Table 1 Phases of thematic analysis
Phase Description of the process
1. Familiarizing yourself
with your data:
Transcribing data (if necessary), reading and re-reading the data, noting down
initial ideas.
2. Generating initial codes: Coding interesting features of the data in a systematic fashion across the entire
data set, collating data relevant to each code.
3. Searching for themes: Collating codes into potential themes, gathering all data relevant to each
potential theme.
4. Reviewing themes: Checking if the themes work in relation to the coded extracts (Level 1) and the
entire data set (Level 2), generating a thematic ‘map’ of the analysis.
5. Defining and naming
themes:
Ongoing analysis to refine the specifics of each theme, and the overall story the
analysis tells, generating clear definitions and names for each theme.
6. Producing the report: The final opportunity for analysis. Selection of vivid, compelling extract
examples, final analysis of selected extracts, relating back of the analysis to the
research question and literature, producing a scholarly report of the analysis.
Using thematic analysis in psychology 87
meanings are created, rather than simply a
mechanical act of putting spoken sounds on
paper (Lapadat and Lindsay, 1999).
Various conventions exist for transforming
spoken texts into written texts (see Edwards
and Lampert, 1993; Lapadat and Lindsay,
1999). Some systems of transcription have
been developed for specific forms of analysis
�/ such as the ‘Jefferson’ system for CA (see
Atkinson and Heritage, 1984; Hutchby and
Wooffitt, 1998). However, thematic analysis,
even constructionist thematic analysis, does
not require the same level of detail in the
transcript as conversation, discourse or even
narrative analysis. As there is no one way to
conduct thematic analysis, there is no one set
of guidelines to follow when producing a
transcript. However, at a minimum it re-
quires a rigorous and thorough ‘ortho-
graphic’ transcript �/ a ‘verbatim’ account of
all verbal (and sometimes nonverbal �/ eg,
coughs) utterances.8 What is important is
that the transcript retains the information
you need, from the verbal account, and in a
way which is ‘true’ to its original nature (eg,
punctuation added can alter the meaning of
data �/ for example ‘I hate it, you know. I do’
versus ‘I hate it. You know I do’, Poland,
2002: 632), and that the transcription con-
vention is practically suited to the purpose of
analysis (Edwards, 1993).
As we have noted, the time spent in
transcription is not wasted, as it informs
the early stages of analysis, and you will
develop a far more thorough understanding
of your data through having transcribed it.
Furthermore, the close attention needed to
transcribe data may facilitate the close read-
ing and interpretative skills needed to ana-
lyse the data (Lapadat and Lindsay, 1999). If
your data have already been, or will be,
transcribed for you, it is important that you
spend more time familiarising yourself with
the data, and also check the transcripts back
against the original audio recordings for
‘accuracy’ (as should always be done).
Phase 2: generating initial codes
Phase 2 begins when you have read and
familiarized yourself with the data, and have
generated an initial list of ideas about what
is in the data and what is interesting about
them. This phase then involves the produc-
tion of initial codes from the data. Codes
identify a feature of the data (semantic
content or latent) that appears interesting
to the analyst, and refer to ‘the most basic
segment, or element, of the raw data or
information that can be assessed in a mean-
ingful way regarding the phenomenon’
(Boyatzis, 1998: 63). See Figure 1 for an
example of codes applied to a short segment
of data. The process of coding is part of
analysis (Miles and Huberman, 1994), as you
are organising your data into meaningful
groups (Tuckett, 2005). However, your
coded data differ from the units of analysis
(your themes), which are (often) broader.
Your themes, which you start to develop in
the next phase, are where the interpretative
analysis of the data occurs, and in relation to
which arguments about the phenomenon
being examined are made (Boyatzis, 1998).
Coding will, to some extent, depend on
whether the themes are more ‘data-driven’
or ‘theory-driven’ �/ in the former, the
Data extract Coded for
it's too much like hard work I mean how much paper have you got to sign
to change a flippin’ name no I I mean no I no we we have thought about it
((inaudible)) half heartedly and thought no no I jus- I can’t be bothered,
it’s too much like hard work. (Kate F07a)
1. Talked about with partner
2. Too much hassle to change name
Figure 1 Data extract, with codes applied (from Clarke et al ., 2006)
88 V Braun and V Clarke
themes will depend on the data, but in the
latter, you might approach the data with
specific questions in mind that you wish to
code around. It will also depend on whether
you are aiming to code the content of the
entire data set, or whether you are coding to
identify particular (and possibly limited)
features of the data set. Coding can be
performed either manually or through a
software programme (see, eg, Kelle, 2004;
Seale, 2000, for discussion of software
programmes).
Work systematically through the entire
data set, giving full and equal attention to
each data item, and identify interesting
aspects in the data items that may form
the basis of repeated patterns (themes)
across the data set. There are a number of
ways of actually coding extracts. If coding
manually, you can code your data by writ-
ing notes on the texts you are analysing,
by using highlighters or coloured pens to
indicate potential patterns, or by using
‘post-it’ notes to identify segments of data.
You may initially identify the codes, and
then match them with data extracts that
demonstrate that code, but it is important in
this phase to ensure that all actual data
extracts are coded, and then collated to-
gether within each code. This may involve
copying extracts of data from individual
transcripts or photocopying extracts of
printed data, and collating each code to-
gether in separate computer files or using
file cards. If using computer software, you
code by tagging and naming selections of
text within each data item.
Key advice for this phase is: (a) code for as
many potential themes/patterns as possible
(time permitting) �/ you never know what
might be interesting later; (b) code extracts
of data inclusively �/ ie, keep a little of the
surrounding data if relevant, a common
criticism of coding is that the context is
lost (Bryman, 2001); and (c) remember that
you can code individual extracts of data in
as many different ‘themes’ as they fit into �/
so an extract may be uncoded, coded once,
or coded many times, as relevant. Note that
no data set is without contradiction, and a
satisfactory thematic ‘map’ that you will
eventually produce �/ an overall conceptua-
lization of the data patterns, and relation-
ships between them9 �/ does not have to
smooth out or ignore the tensions and
inconsistencies within and across data
items. It is important to retain accounts
that depart from the dominant story in the
analysis, so do not ignore these in your
coding.
Phase 3: searching for themes
Phase 3 begins when all data have been
initially coded and collated, and you have a
long list of the different codes that you have
identified across the data set. This phase,
which re-focuses the analysis at the broader
level of themes, rather than codes, involves
sorting the different codes into potential
themes, and collating all the relevant coded
data extracts within the identified themes.
Essentially, you are starting to analyse your
codes and consider how different codes
may combine to form an overarching theme.
It may be helpful at this phase to use visual
representations to help you sort the differ-
ent codes into themes. You might use tables,
or mind-maps, or write the name each code
(and a brief description) on a separate piece
of paper and play around with organizing
them into theme-piles. A thematic map of
this early stage can be seen in Figure 2 (the
examples in Figures 2�/4 come from the
analysis presented in Braun and Wilkinson,
2003 of women’s talk about the vagina).
This is when you start thinking about the
relationship between codes, between
themes, and between different levels of
themes (eg, main overarching themes and
Using thematic analysis in psychology 89
sub-themes within them). Some initial
codes may go on to form main themes,
whereas others may form sub-themes, and
others still may be discarded. At this stage,
you may also have a set of codes that do not
seem to belong anywhere, and it is perfectly
acceptable to create a ‘theme’ called ‘mis-
cellaneous’ to house the codes �/ possibly
temporarily �/ that do not seem to fit into
your main themes.
You end this phase with a collection of
candidate themes, and sub-themes, and all
extracts of data that have been coded in
relation to them. At this point, you will start
to have a sense of the significance of
individual themes. However, do not aban-
don anything at this stage, as without
looking at all the extracts in detail (the
next phase) it is uncertain whether the
themes hold as they are, or whether some
Figure 2 Initial thematic map, showing five main themes (final analysis presented in Braun and
Wilkinson, 2003)
Figure 3 Developed thematic map, showing three main themes (final analysis presented in Braun and
Wilkinson, 2003)
90 V Braun and V Clarke
need to be combined, refined and separated,
or discarded.
Phase 4: reviewing themes
Phase 4 begins when you have devised a set
of candidate themes, and it involves the
refinement of those themes. During this
phase, it will become evident that some
candidate themes are not really themes (eg,
if there are not enough data to support them,
or the data are too diverse), while others
might collapse into each other (eg, two
apparently separate themes might form
one theme). Other themes might need
to be broken down into separate themes.
Patton’s (1990) for dual criteria judging
categories �/ internal homogeneity and ex-
ternal heterogeneity �/ are worth consider-
ing here. Data within themes should cohere
together meaningfully, while there should
be clear and identifiable distinctions be-
tween themes.
This phase involves two levels of review-
ing and refining your themes. Level one
involves reviewing at the level of the coded
data extracts. This means you need to read
all the collated extracts for each theme, and
consider whether they appear to form a
coherent pattern. If your candidate themes
do appear to form a coherent pattern, you
then move on to the second level of this
phase. If your candidate themes do not fit,
you will need to consider whether the
theme itself is problematic, or whether
some of the data extracts within it simply
do not fit there �/ in which case, you would
rework your theme, creating a new theme,
finding a home for those extracts that do not
currently work in an already-existing
theme, or discarding them from the analy-
sis. Once you are satisfied that your candi-
date themes adequately capture the
contours of the coded data �/ once you
have a candidate ‘thematic map’ �/ you
are ready to move on to level two of this
phase. The outcome of this refinement
process can be seen in the thematic map
presented in Figure 3.
Level two involves a similar process, but
in relation to the entire data set. At this
level, you consider the validity of indivi-
dual themes in relation to the data set, but
also whether your candidate thematic map
‘accurately’ reflects the meanings evident in
the data set as a whole. To some extent,
what counts as ‘accurate representation’
depends on your theoretical and analytic
approach. However, in this phase you re-
read your entire data set for two purposes.
The first is, as discussed, to ascertain
whether the themes ‘work’ in relation to
the data set. The second is to code any
additional data within themes that has been
missed in earlier coding stages. The need for
re-coding from the data set is to be expected
as coding is an ongoing organic process.
If the thematic map works, then you
moves on to the next phase. However, if
the map does not fit the data set, you need
to return to further reviewing and refining
of your coding until you have devised a
Figure 4 Final thematic map, showing final two main themes (see Braun and Wilkinson, 2003).
Using thematic analysis in psychology 91
satisfactory thematic map. In so doing, it is
possible that you will identify potential
new themes, and you will need to start
coding for these as well, if they are of
interest and relevent. However, a word
of warning: as coding data and generating
themes could go on ad infinitum , it is
important not to get over-enthusiastic with
endless re-coding. It is impossible to pro-
vide clear guidelines on when to stop, but
when your refinements are not adding any-
thing substantial, stop! If the process of
recoding is only fine-tuning and making
more nuanced a coding frame that already
works �/ ie, it fits the data well �/ recognize
this and stop. Consider it as similar to
editing written work �/ you could endlessly
edit your sentences and paragraphs, but
after a few editing turns, any further work
is usually unnecessary refinement �/ similar
to rearranging the hundreds and thousands
on an already nicely decorated cake.
At the end of this phase, you should have
a fairly good idea of what your different
themes are, how they fit together, and the
overall story they tell about the data.
Phase 5: defining and naming themes
Phase 5 begins when you have a satisfactory
thematic map of your data �/ see Figure 4 for
the final refinements of Virginia’s thematic
map. At this point, you then define and
further refine the themes you will present
for your analysis, and analyse the data
within them. By ‘define and refine’, we
mean identifying the ‘essence’ of what
each theme is about (as well as the themes
overall), and determining what aspect of the
data each theme captures. It is important
not to try and get a theme to do too much, or
to be too diverse and complex. You do this
by going back to collated data extracts for
each theme, and organizing them into a
coherent and internally consistent account,
with accompanying narrative. It is vital that
you do not just paraphrase the content of
the data extracts presented, but identify
what is of interest about them and why.
For each individual theme, you need to
conduct and write a detailed analysis. As
well as identifying the ‘story’ that each
theme tells, it is important to consider
how it fits into the broader overall ‘story’
that you are telling about your data, in
relation to the research question or ques-
tions, to ensure there is not too much
overlap between themes. So it is necessary
to consider the themes themselves, and
each theme in relation to the others. As
part of the refinement, you will need to
identify whether or not a theme contains
any sub-themes. Sub-themes are essentially
themes-within-a-theme. They can be useful
for giving structure to a particularly large
and complex theme, and also for demon-
strating the hierarchy of meaning within the
data. For instance, in one of Virginia’s
analyses of women’s talk about the vagina,
she identified two overarching themes in
women’s talk: the vagina as liability, and the
vagina as asset (Braun and Wilkinson,
2003). Within each theme, three sub-themes
were identified: for liability the sub-themes
were ‘nastiness and dirtiness’, ‘anxieties’
and ‘vulnerability’; for asset the sub-themes
were ‘satisfaction’, ‘power’ and ‘pleasure’.
However, these eventual final themes and
sub-themes resulted from a process of re-
finement of initial themes and sub-themes,
as shown in Figures 2�/4.
It is important that by the end of this phase
you can clearly define what your themes are
and what they are not. One test for this is to
see whether you can describe the scope and
content of each theme in a couple of sen-
tences. If not, further refinement of that
theme may be needed. Although you will
already have given your themes working
titles, this is also the point to start thinking
92 V Braun and V Clarke
about the names you will give them in the
final analysis. Names need to be concise,
punchy, and immediately give the reader a
sense of what the theme is about.
Phase 6: producing the report
Phase 6 begins when you have a set of fully
worked-out themes, and involves the final
analysis and write-up of the report. The
task of the write-up of a thematic anal-
ysis, whether it is for publication or for a
research assignment or dissertation, is to
tell the complicated story of your data in a
way which convinces the reader of the merit
and validity of your analysis. It is important
that the analysis (the write-up of it, includ-
ing data extracts) provides a concise, coher-
ent, logical, non-repetitive and interesting
account of the story the data tell �/ within
and across themes. Your write-up must
provide sufficient evidence of the themes
within the data �/ ie, enough data extracts to
demonstrate the prevalence of the theme.
Choose particularly vivid examples, or ex-
tracts which capture the essence of the
point you are demonstrating, without un-
necessary complexity. The extract should be
easily identifiable as an example of the
issue. However, your write-up needs to do
more than just provide data. Extracts need
to be embedded within an analytic narrative
that compellingly illustrates the story you
are telling about your data, and your analy-
tic narrative needs to go beyond description
of the data, and make an argument in
relation to your research question.
Pinning down what interpretative
analysis actually entails
It is difficult to specify exactly what inter-
pretative analysis actually entails, particu-
larly as the specifics of it will vary from
study to study. As a first step, we recom-
mend looking at published examples of
thematic analysis, particularly of the speci-
fic version you are planning to use (this is
made somewhat more difficult in that the-
matic analysis is often not a named method,
but you can find examples, eg, Ellis and
Kitzinger, 2002; Kitzinger and Willmott,
2002; Toerien and Wilkinson, 2004). In
order to provide a sense of the sorts of
questions you should be asking of your data,
and the sorts of analytic claims you should
be seeking to make, we will discuss a
particularly good example of an inductive
thematic analysis, which emphasizes un-
derstanding men’s experiences in relation to
the broader social context (see Frith and
Gleeson, 2004).
Frith and Gleeson (2004) aim to ex-
plore how men’s feelings about their
bodies influence their clothing practices,
and they use data gathered in qualitative
questionnaires from 75 men to answer
this question. They report four themes:
practicality of clothing choices; lack of
concern about appearance; use of cloth-
ing to conceal or reveal the body; use of
clothing to fit cultural ideals. Each theme
is clearly linked back to the overall
research question, but each is distinct.
They provide a clear sense of the scope
and diversity of each theme, using a
combination of analyst narrative and
illustrative data extracts. Where relevant,
they broaden their analysis out, moving
from a descriptive to an interpretative
level (often relating their claims to exist-
ing literature). For example, in ‘men
value practicality’, they make sense of
men’s accounts in relation to gender
norms and stereotypes, linking the ac-
counts individual men provided to the
expectations that men �/ as members of
society �/ face. What they do, as analysts,
Using thematic analysis in psychology 93
is relate the patterns of meaning in men’s
responses to an academic analysis of how
gender operates. In so doing, they de-
monstrate the dual position that analysts
need to take: as both cultural members
and cultural commentators . Their ‘discus-
sion’ section makes broader analytic
statements about the overall story that
the themes tell us about men’s relation-
ship with clothing. This story reveals
that men ‘deliberately and strategically
use clothing to manipulate their appear-
ance to meet cultural ideals of masculi-
nity’ (Frith and Gleeson, 2004: 45), in a
way more traditionally associated with
women. This analysis makes an impor-
tant contribution in that it challenges
perceived wisdom about clothing/appear-
ance and masculinity.
As this example demonstrates, your ana-
lytic claims need to be grounded in, but go
beyond, the ‘surface’ of the data, even for a
‘semantic’ level analysis. The sort of ques-
tions you need to be asking, towards the end
phases of your analysis, include: ‘What does
this theme mean?’ ‘What are the assump-
tions underpinning it?’ ‘What are the im-
plications of this theme?’ ‘What conditions
are likely to have given rise to it?’ ‘Why
do people talk about this thing in this
particular way (as opposed to other
ways)?’ and ‘What is the overall story the
different themes reveal about the topic?’.
These sorts of questions should guide the
analysis once you have a clear sense of your
thematic map.
Potential pitfalls to avoid when doing
thematic analysis
Thematic analysis is a relatively straight-
forward form of qualitative analysis, which
does not require the same detailed theore-
tical and technical knowledge that ap-
proaches such as DA or CA do. It is
relatively easy to conduct a good thematic
analysis on qualitative data, even when you
are still learning qualitative techniques.
However, there are a number of things that
can result in a poor analysis. In this section
we identify these potential pitfalls, in the
hope that they can be avoided.
The first of these is a failure to actually
analyse the data at all! Thematic analysis is
not just a collection of extracts strung
together with little or no analytic narrative.
Nor is it a selection of extracts with analytic
comment that simply or primarily para-
phrases their content. The extracts in the-
matic analysis are illustrative of the analytic
points the researcher makes about the data,
and should be used to illustrate/support an
analysis that goes beyond their specific
content, to make sense of the data, and tell
the reader what it does or might mean �/ as
discussed above. A second, associated pit-
fall is the using of the data collection
questions (such as from an interview sche-
dule) as the ‘themes’ that are reported. In
such a case, no analytic work has been
carried out to identify themes across the
entire data set, or make sense of the pattern-
ing of responses.
The third is a weak or unconvincing
analysis, where the themes do not appear
to work, where there is too much overlap
between themes, or where the themes are
not internally coherent and consistent. All
aspects of the theme should cohere around a
central idea or concept. This pitfall has
occurred if, depending on what the analysis
is trying to do, it fails adequately to capture
the majority of the data, or fails to provide a
rich description/interpretation of one or
more aspects of the data. A weak or un-
94 V Braun and V Clarke
convincing analysis can also stem from a
failure to provide adequate examples from
the data �/ for example, only one or two
extracts for a theme. This point is essen-
tially about the rhetorics of presentation,
and the need for the analysis to be convin-
cing to someone who has not read the entire
data set: ‘The ‘‘analysis’’ of the material. . . is
a deliberate and self-consciously artful crea-
tion by the researcher, and must be con-
structed to persuade the reader of the
plausibility of an argument’ (Foster and
Parker, 1995: 204). In so doing, one avoids
(the appearance of) what Bryman (1988) has
referred to as ‘anecdotalism’ in qualitative
research �/ where one or a few instances of a
phenomenon are reified into a pattern or
theme, when it or they are actually idiosyn-
cratic. This is not to say that a few instances
cannot be of interest, or revealing; but it is
important not to misrepresent them as an
overarching theme.
The fourth pitfall is a mismatch between
the data and the analytic claims that are
made about it. In such an (unfounded)
analysis, the claims cannot be supported
by the data, or, in the worst case, the data
extracts presented suggest another analysis
or even contradict the claims. The re-
searcher needs to make sure that their
interpretations and analytic points are con-
sistent with the data extracts. A weak
analysis does not appear to consider other
obvious alternative readings of the data, or
fails to consider variation (and even contra-
diction) in the account that is produced. A
pattern in data is rarely, if ever, going to be
100% complete and non-contradicted, so an
analysis which suggests that it is, without a
thorough explanation, is open to suspicion.
It is important to pick compelling examples
to demonstrate the themes, so give this
considerable thought.
The fifth involves a mismatch between
theory and analytic claims, or between the
research questions and the form of thematic
analysis used. A good thematic analysis
needs to make sure that the interpretations
of the data are consistent with the theoretical
framework. So, for instance, if you are work-
ing within an experiential framework, you
would typically not make claims about the
social construction of the research topic, and
if you were doing constructionist thematic
analysis, you would not treat people’s talk
of experience as a transparent window on
their world. Finally, even a good and inter-
esting analysis which fails to spell out
its theoretical assumptions, or clarify how it
was undertaken, and for what purpose, is
lacking crucial information (Holloway and
Todres, 2003), and thus fails in one aspect.
What makes good thematic analysis?
One of the criticisms of qualitative research
from those outside the field is the percep-
tion that ‘anything goes’. For instance, this
sentiment is echoed in the first sentence of
Laubschagne’s (2003) abstract: ‘For many
scientists used to doing quantitative studies
the whole concept of qualitative research is
unclear, almost foreign, or ‘‘airy fairy’’ �/ not
‘‘real’’ research.’ However, although ‘quali-
tative’ research cannot be subjected to the
same criteria as ‘quantitative’ approaches, it
does provide methods of analysis that
should be applied rigorously to the data.
Furthermore, criteria for conducting good
qualitative research �/ both data collection
and analysis �/ do exist (eg, Elliott et al .,
1999; Parker, 2004; Seale, 1999; Silverman,
2000; Yardley, 2000). The British Psycholo-
gical Society offers relatively succinct on-
line guidelines for assessing quality in qua-
litative research (see http://www.bps.org.
Using thematic analysis in psychology 95
uk/publications/journals/joop/qualitative-
guidelines.cfm). ‘Criteria’ for assessing qua-
litative research is a not uncontroversial
topic, with concerns raised about rigid
criteria limiting freedom and stifling meth-
odological development (Elliott et al ., 1999;
Parker, 2004; Reicher, 2000). Reicher (2000)
takes the critique further, by asking whether
the incredibly diverse range of qualitative
approaches can and should be subject to
the same criteria.
Bracketing these critiques off, the issues
raised in many general qualitative research
assessment criteria can be more or less
applied to thematic forms of analysis. As
thematic analysis is a flexible method, you
also need to be clear and explicit about what
you are doing, and what you say you are
doing needs to match up with what you
actually do. In this sense, the theory and
method need to be applied rigorously, and
‘rigour lies in devising a systematic method
whose assumptions are congruent with the
way one conceptualizes the subject matter’
(Reicher and Taylor, 2005: 549). A concise
checklist of criteria to consider when deter-
mining whether you have generated a good
thematic analysis is provided in Table 2.
So what does thematic analysis offer
psychologists?
We now end this paper with some brief
comments on the advantages and disadvan-
tages of thematic analysis. As we have
shown throughout this paper, thematic ana-
lysis is not a complex method. Indeed, as
you can see from Table 3, its advantages are
many. However, it is not without some
disadvantages, which we will now briefly
consider. Many of the disadvantages de-
pend more on poorly conducted analyses
or inappropriate research questions than on
Table 2 A 15-point checklist of criteria for good thematic analysis
Process No. Criteria
Transcription 1 The data have been transcribed to an appropriate level of detail, and the transcripts
have been checked against the tapes for ‘accuracy’.
Coding 2 Each data item has been given equal attention in the coding process.
3 Themes have not been generated from a few vivid examples (an anecdotal approach),
but instead the coding process has been thorough, inclusive and comprehensive.
4 All relevant extracts for all each theme have been collated.
5 Themes have been checked against each other and back to the original data set.
6 Themes are internally coherent, consistent, and distinctive.
Analysis 7 Data have been analysed �/ interpreted, made sense of �/ rather than just paraphrased
or described.
8 Analysis and data match each other �/ the extracts illustrate the analytic claims.
9 Analysis tells a convincing and well-organized story about the data and topic.
10 A good balance between analytic narrative and illustrative extracts is provided.
Overall 11 Enough time has been allocated to complete all phases of the analysis adequately,
without rushing a phase or giving it a once-over-lightly.
Written report 12 The assumptions about, and specific approach to, thematic analysis are clearly
explicated.
13 There is a good fit between what you claim you do, and what you show you have
done �/ ie, described method and reported analysis are consistent.
14 The language and concepts used in the report are consistent with the epistemological
position of the analysis.
15 The researcher is positioned as active in the research process; themes do not just
‘emerge’.
96 V Braun and V Clarke
the method itself. Further, the flexibility of
the method �/ which allows for a wide range
of analytic options �/ means that the poten-
tial range of things that can be said about
your data is broad. While this is an advan-
tage, it can also be a disadvantage in that it
makes developing specific guidelines for
higher-phase analysis difficult, and can be
potentially paralysing to the researcher try-
ing to decide what aspects of their data to
focus on. Another issue to consider is that a
thematic analysis has limited interpretative
power beyond mere description if it is not
used within an existing theoretical frame-
work that anchors the analytic claims that
are made.
Other disadvantages appear when the-
matic analysis is considered in relation to
some of the other qualitative analytic meth-
ods. For instance, unlike narrative or other
biographical approaches, you are unable to
retain a sense of continuity and contradic-
tion through any one individual account,
and these contradictions and consistencies
across individual accounts may be reveal-
ing. In contrast to methods similar to DA
and CA, a simple thematic analysis does not
allow the researcher to make claims about
language use, or the fine-grained function-
ality of talk.
Finally, it is worth noting that thematic
analysis currently has no particular kudos
as an analytic method �/ this, we argue,
stems from the very fact that it is poorly
demarcated and claimed, yet widely used.
This means that thematic analysis is fre-
quently, or appears to be, what is simply
carried out by someone without the knowl-
edge or skills to perform a supposedly more
sophisticated �/ certainly more kudos-bear-
ing �/ ‘branded’ form of analysis like
grounded theory, IPA or DA. We hope this
paper will change this view as, we argue, a
rigorous thematic approach can produce an
insightful analysis that answers particular
research questions. What is important is
choosing a method that is appropriate to
your research question, rather than falling
victim to ‘methodolatry’, where you are
committed to method rather than topic/
content or research questions (Holloway
and Todres, 2003). Indeed, your method of
analysis should be driven by both your
research question and your broader theore-
tical assumptions. As we have demon-
strated, thematic analysis is a flexible
approach that can be used across a range
of epistemologies and research questions.
Notes
1. Boyatzis (1998) provides a much more
detailed account of thematic analysis. However,
we do not feel that it is a particularly accessible
account for those unfamiliar with qualitative
approaches. Moreover, his approach differs
from ours in that, although he acknowledges
the subjective dimension of qualitative analysis,
his approach is ultimately, if often implicitly,
located within a positivist empiricist paradigm.
2. Dey’s (1993) account of on ‘qualitative data
analysis’, which aims to identify shared techni-
ques across the diverse range of qualitative
Table 3 Advantages of thematic analysis
Flexibility.
Relatively easy and quick method to learn, and do.
Accessible to researchers with little or no experience of
qualitative research.
Results are generally accessible to educated general
public.
Useful method for working within participatory re-
search paradigm, with participants as collaborators.
Can usefully summarize key features of a large body of
data, and/or offer a ‘thick description’ of the data set.
Can highlight similarities and differences across the
data set.
Can generate unanticipated insights.
Allows for social as well as psychological interpreta-
tions of data.
Can be useful for producing qualitative analyses suited
to informing policy development.
Using thematic analysis in psychology 97
methods, and demonstrate how to do ‘qualitative
analysis’, reinforces this point in that his focus is
largely thematic �/ but not claimed as such.
3. Some authors, such as Potter (1997: 147�/
48) argue that one should not simply provide
‘recipes’ for qualitative methods, such as DA,
because ‘a large part of doing discourse analysis
is a craft skill, more like bike riding or sexing a
chicken than following the recipe for a mild
chicken rogan josh. . . This makes it hard to
describe and learn’. While we do not disagree
that the skills needed for qualitative analyses of
all types need to be learned, others, such as
McLeod (2001), argue that by not discussing the
‘how to’ of analysis, we keep certain methods
mysterious (and thus elitist). Instead, if we want
to make methods democratic and accessible �/
and indeed, to make qualitative research of all
forms more understandable to those not trained
in the methods, and arguably thus more popular
�/ we need to provide concrete advice on how to
actually do it. We are not questioning the
importance of ‘non-recipe’ forms of training,
but while ‘recipes’ necessarily diminish the
complexity of certain methods, they are impor-
tant for making methods accessible.
4. Foster and Parker (1995) suggest one
way to acknowledge the creative and active role
of the analyst is to use the first person when
writing.
5. Content analysis is another method that
can be used to identify patterns across qualitative
data, and is sometimes treated as similar to
thematic approaches (eg, Wilkinson, 2000). How-
ever, content analysis tends to focus at a more
micro level, often provides (frequency) counts
(Wilkinson, 2000), and allows for quantitative
analyses of initially qualitative data (Ryan and
Bernard, 2000). Thematic analysis differs from
this in that themes tend not to be quantified
(although sometimes they may be; and Boyatzis
(1998) suggests thematic analysis can be used to
transform qualitative data into a quantitative
form, and subject them to statistical analyses;
and the unit of analysis tends to be more than a
word or phrase, which it typically is in content
analysis.
6. The definition by Boyatzis (1998) of latent
and manifest is somewhat narrower than our
identification of latent and semantic, and he
identifies thematic analysis as incorporating
both latent and manifest aspects. However, this
results from the fact that he associates the
process of interpretation with latent analysis �/
whereas we would argue that it should also be an
important element of a semantic approach.
7. We are assuming that you will be working
with a ‘good quality’ data corpus and data set.
We would argue that ‘good data’ are defined by a
particular set of criteria regarding what, why,
and how they were collected, and offer rich,
detailed and complex accounts of the topic.
Good data do not just provide a surface over-
view of the topic of interest, or simply reiterate a
commonsense account. The challenge for the
novice researcher is to interact with research
participants in such a way that they generate
rich and complex insights. Producing a good
analysis of poor quality data is a far more
demanding task for the analyst, although it can
potentially be performed by a skilled and ex-
perienced analyst.
8. See Poland (2002) for a discussion of the
problems with the idea of a ‘verbatim’ transcript,
and what is left out, and retained, through this
process.
9. What we mean by thematic map is similar
to, but less detailed than, the ‘codebook’ Ryan
and Bernard (2000) refer to, which involves a
detailed account of the hierarchical relationship
between codes, as well as a description of each,
their criteria, exemplars and counter �/ examples,
and other such details. Like Boyatzis’s (1998)
account of a thematic code, this model is then
applied to (and revised in relation to) the data.
See Figures 2�/4 for visual representations of a
thematic maps and its refinement. Another ex-
ample of a thematic map �/ this time in table
form �/ can be found in Frith and Gleeson (2004).
References
Antaki, C., Billig, M., Edwards, D. and Potter, J.
2002: Discourse analysis means doing ana-
lysis: a critique of six analytic shortcomings.
98 V Braun and V Clarke
DAOL Discourse Analysis Online [electronic
version], 1(1).
Aronson, J. 1994: A pragmatic view of thematic
analysis. Qualitative Report 2(1).
Atkinson, J.M. and Heritage, J. 1984: Structures
of social action: studies in conversation
analysis . Cambridge University Press.
Attride-Stirling, J. 2001: Thematic networks: an
analytic tool for qualitative research. Quali-
tative Research 1, 385�/405.
Bird, C.M. 2005: How I stopped dreading and
learned to love transcription. Qualitative
Inquiry 11, 226�/48.
Boyatzis, R.E. 1998: Transforming qualitative
information: thematic analysis and code
development . Sage.
Braun, V. 2005a: In search of (better) female
sexual pleasure: female genital ‘cosmetic’
surgery. Sexualities 8, 407�/24.
Braun, V. 2005b: Selling the perfect vulva. Manu-
script under submission.
Braun, V. and Kitzinger, C. 2001: The perfectible
vagina: size matters. Culture Health & Sexu-
ality 3, 263�/77.
Braun, V. and Wilkinson, S. 2003: Liability or
asset? Women talk about the vagina. Psycho-
logy of Women Section Review 5, 28�/42.
Braun, V., Gavey, N. and McPhillips, K. 2003: The
‘fair deal?’ Unpacking accounts of recipro-
city in heterosex. Sexualities 6, 237�/61.
Bryman, A. 1988: Quantity and quality in social
research . Routledge.
Bryman, A. 2001: Social research methods .
Oxford University Press.
Burman, E. and Parker, I., editors, 1993: Dis-
course analytic research: repertoires and
readings of texts in action . Routledge.
Burr, V. 1995: An introduction to social con-
structionism . Routledge.
Charmaz, K. 2002: Qualitative interviewing and
grounded theory analysis. In Gubrium, J.F.
and Holstein J.A., editors, Handbook of
interview research: context and method .
Sage, 675�/94.
Clarke, V. 2005: ‘We’re all very liberal in our
views’: Students’ talk about lesbian and gay
parenting. Lesbian and Gay Psychology Re-
view 6, 2�/15.
Clarke, V. and Kitzinger, C. 2004: Lesbian and gay
parents on talk shows: resistance or collu-
sion in heterosexism. Qualitative Research
in Psychology 1, 195�/217.
Clarke, V., Burns, M. and Burgoyne, C. 2006:
‘Who would take whose name?’ An explora-
tory study of naming practices in same-sex
relationships. Manuscript under submission.
Dey, I. 1993: Qualitative data analysis: a user-
friendly guide for social scientists . Routledge.
Edwards, J.A. 1993: Principles and contrasting
systems of discourse transcription. In Ed-
wards, J.A. and Lampert, M.D., editors,
Talking data: transcription and coding in
discourse research. Lawrence Erlbaum As-
sociates, 3�/31.
Edwards, J.A. and Lampert, M.D., editors, 1993:
Talking data: transcription and coding in
discourse research . Lawrence Erlbaum As-
sociates.
Elliott, R., Fischer, C.T. and Rennie, D.L. 1999:
Evolving guidelines for publication of qua-
litative research studies in psychology and
related fields. British Journal of Clinical
Psychology 38, 215�/29.
Ellis, S.J. and Kitzinger, C. 2002: Denying equal-
ity: an analysis of arguments against low-
ering the age of consent for sex between
men. Journal of Community and Applied
Social Psychology 12, 167�/80.
Ely, M., Vinz, R., Downing, M. and Anzul, M.
1997: On writing qualitative research: living
by words . Routledge/Falmer.
Fine, M. 2002: Disruptive voices: the possibilities
for feminist research . University of Michi-
gan Press.
Foster, J.J. and Parker, I. 1995: Carrying out
investigations in psychology: methods and
statistics . BPS Books.
Frith, H. and Gleeson, K. 2004: Clothing and
embodiment: men managing body image and
Using thematic analysis in psychology 99
appearance. Psychology of Men and Mascu-
linity 5, 40�/48.
Glaser, B. 1992: Basics of grounded theory
analysis . Sociology Press.
Hayes, N. 1997: Theory-led thematic analysis:
social identification in small companies. In
Hayes, N., editor, Doing qualitative analysis
in psychology. Psychology Press.
Holloway, I. and Todres, L. 2003: The status of
method: flexibility, consistency and coher-
ence. Qualitative Research 3, 345�/57.
Hollway, W. 1989: Subjectivity and method in
psychology: gender, meaning and science .
Sage.
Hollway, W. and Jefferson, T. 2000: Doing quali-
tative research differently: free association,
narrative and the interview method . Sage.
Hutchby, I. and Wooffitt, R. 1998: Conversation
analysis: principles, practices and applica-
tions . Polity Press.
Kelle, U. 2004: Computer-assisted analysis of
qualitative data. In Flick, U., von Kardorff,
E. and Steinke, I., editors, A companion to
qualitative research . Sage, 276�/83.
Kitzinger, C. and Willmott, J. 2002: ‘The thief of
womanhood’: women’s experience of poly-
cystic ovarian syndrome. Social Science &
Medicine 54, 349�/61.
Lapadat, J.C. and Lindsay, A.C. 1999: Transcrip-
tion in research and practice: from standar-
dization of technique to interpretive posi-
tionings. Qualitative Inquiry 5, 64�/86.
Laubschagne, A. 2003: Qualitative research �/
airy fairy or fundamental? Qualitative Re-
port [electronic version], 8(1).
McLeod, J. 2001: Qualitative research in counsel-
ling and psychotherapy. Sage.
Meehan, T., Vermeer, C. and Windsor, C. 2000:
Patients’ perceptions of seclusion: a qualita-
tive investigation. Journal of Advanced Nur-
sing 31, 370�/77.
Miles, M.B. and Huberman, A.M. 1994: Qualita-
tive data analysis: an expanded sourcebook ,
second edition. Sage.
Murray, M. 2003: Narrative psychology. In Smith,
J.A., editor, Qualitative psychology: a practi-
cal guide to research methods . Sage, 111�/31.
Parker, I. 2004: Criteria for qualitative research in
psychology. Qualitative Research in Psy-
chology 1, 95�/106.
Patton, M.Q. 1990: Qualitative evaluation and
research methods , second edition. Sage.
Poland, B.D. 2002: Transcription quality. In
Gubrium, J.F. and Holstein, J.A., editors,
Handbook of interview research: context
and method. Sage, 629�/49.
Potter, J. 1997: Discourse analysis as a way of
analysing naturally occurring talk. In Silver-
man, D., editor, Qualitative research: theory,
method and practice . Sage, 144�/60.
Potter, J. and Wetherell, M. 1987: Discourse and
social psychology: beyond attitudes and
behaviour. Sage.
Reicher, S. 2000: Against methodolatry: some
comments on Elliott, Fischer, and Rennie.
British Journal of Clinical Psychology 39, 1�/
6.
Reicher, S. and Taylor, S. 2005: Similarities and
differences between traditions. Psychologist
18, 547�/49.
Riessman, C.K. 1993: Narrative analysis . Sage.
Roulston, K. 2001: Data analysis and ‘theorizing
as ideology’. Qualitative Research 1, 279�/
302.
Rubin, H.J. and Rubin, I.S. 1995: Qualitative
interviewing: the art of hearing data . Sage.
Ryan, G.W. and Bernard, H.R. 2000: Data man-
agement and analysis methods. In Denzin,
N.K. and Lincoln, Y.S., editors, Handbook of
qualitative research , second edition. Sage,
769�/802.
Seale, C. 1999: The quality of qualitative re-
search . Sage.
Seale, C. 2000: Using computers to analyse
qualitative data. In Silverman, D., Doing
qualitative research: a practical handbook .
Sage, 155�/74.
Silverman, D., editor 2000: Doing qualitative
research: a practical handbook . Sage.
100 V Braun and V Clarke
Singer, D. and Hunter, M. 1999: The experience
of premature menopause: a thematic dis-
course analysis. Journal of Reproductive
and Infant Psychology 17, 63�/81.
Smith, J.A. and Osborn, M. 2003: Interpretative
phenomenological analysis. In Smith, J.A.,
editor, Qualitative psychology: a practical
guide to methods . Sage.
Smith, J.A., Jarman, M. and Osborn, M. 1999:
Doing interpretative phenomenological ana-
lysis. In Murray, M. and Chamberlain,
M.M.K., editors, Qualitative health psychol-
ogy: theories and methods . Sage.
Stenner, P. 1993: Discoursing jealousy. In
Burman, E. and Parker, I., editors, Dis-
course analytic research: repertoires and
readings of texts in action. Routledge, 94�/
132.
Strauss, A. and Corbin, J. 1998: Basics of quali-
tative research: techniques and procedures
for developing grounded theory. Sage.
Taylor, G.W. and Ussher, J.M. 2001: Making sense
of S&M: a discourse analytic account. Sex-
ualities 4, 293�/314.
Toerien, M. and Wilkinson, S. 2004: Exploring
the depilation norm: a qualitative question-
naire study of women’s body hair removal.
Qualitative Research in Psychology 1, 69�/
92.
Tuckett, A.G. 2005: Applying thematic analysis
theory to practice: a researcher’s experience.
Contemporary Nurse 19, 75�/87.
Ussher, J.M. and Mooney-Somers, J. 2000: Nego-
tiating desire and sexual subjectivity: narra-
tives of young lesbian avengers. Sexualities
3, 183�/200.
Widdicombe, S. and Wooffitt, R. 1995: The
language of youth subcultures: social iden-
tity in action . Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Wilkinson, S. 2000: Women with breast cancer
talking causes: comparing content, biogra-
phical and discursive analyses. Feminism &
Psychology 10, 431�/60.
Willig, C. 1999: Beyond appearances: a critical
realist approach to social constructionism.
In Nightingale, D.J. and Cromby J., editors,
Social constructionist psychology: a critical
analysis of theory and practice . Open Uni-
versity Press, 37�/51.
Willig, C. 2003: Discourse analysis. In Smith,
J.A., editor, Qualitative psychology: a prac-
tical guide to research methods . Sage, 159�/
83.
Yardley, L. 2000: Dilemmas in qualitative health
research. Psychology and Health 15, 215�/
28.
About the authors
VIRGINIA BRAUN is a senior lecturer in the Department of Psychology at the University
of Auckland, where she teaches, supervises and conducts qualitative research. Her
research interests are primarily focused around women’s health, gendered bodies, and
sex and sexuality, and the intersections between these areas. She is currently working on
projects related to ‘sex in long-term relationships’, ‘female genital cosmetic surgery’ and
‘the social context of STI transmission’.
VICTORIA CLARKE is a senior lecture in social psychology at the University of the West
of England. She has published a number of papers on lesbian and gay parenting, and
co-edited two special issues of Feminism and Psychology on Marriage (with Sara-Jane
Finlay and Sue Wilkinson). She is currently conducting ESRC-funded research on same-
sex relationships (with Carol Burgoyne and Maree Burns) and co-editing (with Elizabeth
Peel) a book on LGBTQ psychology (Out in psychology, Wiley).
Using thematic analysis in psychology 101
ALEXANDER
(2.8)
INT:
right
(0.5)
INT:
that should be us=
AL:
=˚okay˚
(1.4)
INT:
okay
(0.3)
INT:
.hhh so
(.)
INT:
um
(1.9)
INT:
the <way> the interview’s gonna go is
(1.0)
INT:
I’ve got like a: yer know a full on interview schedule here >that I’m gonna follow
and what have you but I’m<
(1.0)
INT:
would rather it sort of went as a conversation than a
(0.2)
INT:
than a sort of question n answer s[essio]n?=
AL:
[yep]
AL:
=˚yea˚
(.)
INT:
so: u:m:
(1.1)
INT:
I’ll refer to it a bit
(0.2)
INT:
but er if we
(0.4)
INT:
chop about n change n n all that sort of stuff I’m I’m really don’t
(0.2)
INT:
.hhh I’m I’m really don’t mind so
(0.8)
INT:
tsk u:m:
(0.3)
INT:
.hhh so we’re looking at ah friendship=
AL:
=m:
(0.6)
INT:
u:m:
(0.3)
INT:
and so a a a good way to start maybe
(0.3)
INT:
would be to ask you to:
(0.4)
INT:
tell me about one of your friends
(0.3)
INT:
u:m:
(0.6)
INT:
<and> sort of tell the story of how you of of how you came to meet them
(0.6)
AL:
right (.) suppose I-
(0.5)
AL:
start with my ah talk about my best friend=
INT:
=okay
(0.3)
AL:
u::m
(0.2)
AL:
mean (.) I used to live in Austria
(0.3)
AL:
for fifteen years and um (.) m parents separated and m my mum took us:
(0.6)
AL:
over to England so:=
INT:
=m:
(0.8)
AL:
quite strange this::
(0.8)
AL:
new big schoo:l I mean I was accepted in Eng- in Austria in (.) basically a ninety-
five percent girls’ school which was heaven for me [there so]
INT:
[ha ha] ha ha [ha ha ha ha]
AL:
[<BRILLIANT>]
AL:
a girl for every day of the year [.hhh] to: u:m being accepted in ((SCHOOL NAME)) which is
INT:
[ha ha]
AL:
an all boys school
(0.5)
INT:
alrigh[t]
AL:
[on]ly sixth form g a allowed to girls so I was like .hhh
(0.3)
AL:
˚no girls˚
(.)
AL:
[so]
INT:
[ha h]a
(0.2)
AL:
it was quite stra:nge quite a new experience s:chool [uniform]
INT:
[how long ago was] this
INT:
ago no[w]
AL:
[ab]out seven years ago coming up seven years
(.)
INT:
so you were sort of in the middle of high school type
(0.2)
INT:
pl[ace xxx]
AL:
[uh u::]m I’d got into year ten (.) I’d fini[she]d (.) e:r one set of school in
INT:
[˚okay˚]
AL:
Austria n
(0.3)
AL:
came into year ten in England which .hhh we never had course work in Eng- in
Austria n stuff like that so it was quite difficult s[o u]:m
INT:
[˚xx˚]
(1.6)
AL:
xxx school uniform we didn’t have so it was all new experiences and
(0.3)
AL:
out of a class of thirty I can’t remember names
(0.8)
AL:
I just didn’t know anyone so
(0.5)
AL:
u:m (.) one of my mate xxx was met him in our history class n xx sat next to him
n he’s now my best friend you know we went out
(0.5)
AL:
to er:m
(0.5)
AL:
school discos n stuff like that and he took me under his wing and (.) yeah
(1.0)
AL:
nice lad
(0.6)
INT:
okay
(0.2)
INT:
so how how
(0.9)
INT:
how how d’you actually:: um
(0.4)
INT:
meet him you met him in class I [guess]
AL:
[in cl]ass yes an[d in] form as well so
INT:
[and so]
(0.4)
INT:
how
(0.3)
INT:
how do you:: sort of um
(2.0)
INT:
>how how do you< sort of pass the time with him you know when when when
you guys are together
(.)
AL:
u:m as in in school or outside
(0.6)
INT:
both I [guess]
AL:
[both] well
(0.5)
AL:
I suppose we just sit about sometimes we also sort of play f footie as you do nd ah
(0.2)
INT:
m:
(0.3)
AL:
u:::m
(2.0)
AL:
he: u:m after our GCSEs xxx ah we I he had a villa in ah Minorca
(0.4)
AL:
a:nd so it was a group of us u:m planning to go out so we >basically< we sat in er
during lunchtime planning what we were going to do and
(0.3)
INT:
˚m˚
(0.9)
AL:
we just sort of went to the pub I mean my mum kicked me out (.) a:t the age of
sixteen n told me to go to the pub with the lads and hh[h >it was like<↑er]: yeah
INT:
[ha ha ha ha]
cheers Mum hhh=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha=
AL:
=.hhh ah: hu:m: (.) so we went (.) to the pub every Friday and
(0.2)
AL:
>e you know< just <general>
(0.3)
AL:
stuff you know just hang around (.) meet up with some girls n stuff
(0.9)
INT:
˚okay˚ (.) and you still you still:
(0.3)
INT:
see him a lot?
(.)
AL:
ah well he’s got up gone up to ((UNIVERSITY NAME)) up there but every time
time he’s down we
(0.3)
AL:
try n meet up for lunch or at least go out for a good old night s so
(0.9)
AL:
yeah we used to go up to Liverpool
(3.0)
AL:
nearly what every other Friday as well (.) before that so
(.)
INT:
okay
(1.2)
INT:
[s]o h his family
AL:
[x]
(0.2)
INT:
are over the water as well are they?
(0.4)
AL:
u::m=
INT:
=or where are they based
(0.6)
AL:
they’re: well they at the moment they’ve got a house in erm America so they are
[in Ameri]ca
INT:
[alright okay]
(0.6)
AL:
so but yeah I went travelling with him as well during my gap year so (.) we spent
what (.) th:ree months
(0.2)
AL:
at least
(0.4)
AL:
twenty-four seven with him so
(.)
INT:
yeah
(0.7)
AL:
normally you you can’t do that with people or (.) I can’t but
(0.5)
AL:
he’s best mate xxx xxx
(0.7)
INT:
why not
(0.5)
INT:
why can’t you
normally with
(0.2)
AL:
I can but you know yh I need I need
other people sometimes it’s just you xx it’s
gotta be someone that you’ve
(0.4)
AL:
want to spen:d
twenty-four seven with you know for a three months knowing
(1.2)
AL:
you’ve gotta travel with them
(0.3)
AL:
<twenty-four seven>
(0.5)
INT:
˚xx[x˚]
AL:
[˚x]xx˚
(1.2)
INT:
it’s quite a big ask=
AL:
=it is [yeah]
INT:
[how’d it] go?
(0.4)
AL:
amazing
(.)
INT:
yea:h
(0.9)
INT:
[did] you ever fight?
AL:
[xx]
(.)
AL:
no
(0.5)
AL:
[not at all]
INT:
[not once]
(0.5)
AL:
not even once
(0.8)
INT:
that’s good
(0.7)
AL:
it’s quite a s:urprise because he’s such an annoying person [sometimes]
INT:
[ha ha] ha ha ha [ha ha]
AL:
[xxx
xxx] during the history lessons he used to poke me continuously in the ah::
shoulders like
(0.1)
INT:
ha ha ha ha ha ha [˚ha] ha˚
AL:
[hhh]
(1.5)
INT:
yea:h that’s the sort of friend you want [ha ha ha ha]
AL:
[m::: he’s a] middle child as well so he
has middle child syndrome
(0.3)
INT:
oh yes=
AL:
=m::=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha
(1.5)
INT:
u:m
(0.8)
INT:
<the>
(0.4)
INT:
your other friends
(0.3)
INT:
is your relationship with them like quite similar to your relationship with this
guy?
(.)
AL:
a:h well we’ve got (.) sort of (.) I’ve got u:m
(0.7)
AL:
there’s a group of the lads
(0.5)
AL:
including a couple of girls but
(0.3)
AL:
they’re part of the lads:=
INT:
=m:=
AL:
=u::m
(0.6)
AL:
yeah there’s sort of (.) I’d say there’s about
(0.2)
AL:
ten of us that go to the pub together but (.) they’re sort of um
(0.4)
AL:
bit older than us qu- some of them
(0.5)
AL:
um sort of there’s
(1.0)
AL:
the four of us ws-
(0.5)
AL:
him
(1.8)
AL:
me
(1.7)
AL:
˚is there three of us? (.) four of us˚ well there’s
(0.5)
AL:
we’re sort of close type of close knitted sometimes when we’re together there’s
(0.3)
AL:
d u:s us three us four
(0.7)
AL:
a:nd then the rest around I mean (.) sort of
(0.3)
AL:
I see
(0.2)
AL:
my <frien:ds>
sort of (.) a as a chosen ↑family you know f you can’t choose y-who your family is but you can choose who your friends are and so .hhh they’re sort of
(.) knitted with me as u:m
(0.8)
AL:
family in a way u::m
(0.4)
AL:
got other sort of
(0.7)
AL:
friends here and there which I’m really close to
(0.6)
AL:
a:nd
(0.2)
AL:
a couple of girls that I’m really close to and I’ve
(0.5)
AL:
jus making new friends at the moment new friend at the moment so
(1.3)
INT:
okay
(0.3)
INT:
right
(0.6)
INT:
so this this new friend how how have you met th[em?]
AL:
[hh]h
(0.2)
AL:
facebook (.) well
(0.2)
INT:
oh yes [xx]
AL:
[over] the internet we u:m I’m going to America
(0.2)
AL:
over the summer
(0.8)
AL:
for th: three
(0.4)
AL:
over just over three months I think
(0.6)
AL:
and I’m I’m going to ↑Camp America for the fir[st] two months
INT:
[˚hm m˚]
(0.8)
AL:
u:m so it’s nice meeting all sorts of people through the:re as well getting to know
them over by m MSN facebook er over the internetand then
(0.4)
AL:
I’ve met this girl who’s erm because I’m going on a trek
(0.4)
AL:
afterwards from er ah New York to LA
(0.6)
AL:
for three weeks which is camping and roughing it and
(0.2)
AL:
it’s gonna be fun and
(.)
INT:
˚coo[l]˚
AL:
[fou]nd that ah they’ve got a blog ↑space on their webpage so I: put on
(0.5)
AL:
saying I’ve created a grou:p in facebook you know let’s get together let’s get to
know each other as we are gonna spend three weeks with each other
(0.4)
INT:
ye:h=
AL:
=a::nd this girl she goes to ((UNIVERSITY NAME)) so we u::m got on really well on MSN you know
(0.2)
AL:
wow er
(0.2)
AL:
I’ve never got on so well so fast with someone (.) just over the internet (.) y’know
(0.5)
AL:
I can trust her I can tell her anything n
(0.3)
AL:
only met her once
(0.4)
AL:
And I met I went er: for dinner with her the other day and
(0.7)
AL:
yeah we get on like a house on fire=
INT:
=yeah?
(.)
INT:
>so how I mean that must have been a bit odd<
(0.6)
INT:
meeting her
(0.3)
INT:
face to face not having:=
AL:
=<yeah it was str[ange> b]ut (.) it’s
INT:
[yeah?]
(0.2)
AL:
quite fast you know (.) got on quite well there’s no [problems at all xxx]
INT:
[cough cou]gh
(1.2)
AL:
pff weird actually
(0.2)
INT:
ha ha ha ha
(1.5)
INT:
okay cool
(0.8)
INT:
u:m
(1.8)
INT:
the
(0.3)
INT:
the first friend you were talking abou[t]
AL:
[m]:
(0.6)
INT:
d’you do you reckon that’s (.) a friend for life I mean [(.) forever]
AL:
[yeah he’s gonna be] my
best man at the wedding if I ha have one so
(0.2)
AL:
definitely=
INT:
=yeah
(.)
AL:
definitely be- er best mate for life
(0.4)
INT:
but is he gonna be going back to the states with his is he going to be mo[ving back
AL:
[ah: no:
INT:
there with his family xxx]
AL:
xxx] his family sort of move about they’re never settled a[nywhere so::]
INT:
[alright okay they
haven’t like] emigrated they’re just (.) working there for a while or
so[mething xxx]
AL:
[well they’ve]
(0.3)
AL:
they’ve got a one year visa (.) out there s- and it depends on the youngest son who’s
(1.3)
AL:
he’s just done his eleven plus so he must be what ten ↑eleven
(0.4)
AL:
and he failed that so they’re hoping he might pass his el er:
(0.8)
AL:
twelve plus so they might go back to England n .hhh (.) they sort of lived in Spain
for ages and (.) moved to England for the schooling and then (.) they liked the
idea of u:m
(0.6)
AL:
America because their oldest son was gonna try out and go to college out ↑there
(0.6)
INT:
˚okay˚
(.)
AL:
u:m but they didn’t take him into the basketball team so
(0.2)
AL:
he went back to England and they’d just bought this house
(0.2)
AL:
just out of random they saw it liked it and just thought oh let’s buy it
(1.2)
AL:
˚s˚
(0.4)
AL:
so:
(.)
INT:
as you do .hhh ha ha ha
(0.2)
AL:
if you’ve got the money why not hhh
(0.2)
INT:
yeah
(0.6)
INT:
so he’s he doesn’t have plans to go over the states with them he’s [gonna]
AL:
[n:]:o
AL:
he[’s g]
INT:
[gonna stick] in the UK
(0.2)
AL:
well
(0.5)
AL:
he doesn’t want to stick in the UK he just wants to (.) he doesn’t like the UK too
much the weather
(0.2)
AL:
doesn’t suit him
(0.4)
AL:
he lived in Spain for so many years s[o:]
INT:
[ye]ah
(0.4)
INT:
yeah
(0.4)
AL:
xxx xxx=
INT:
=>so how do you think< I mean: if he (.) heads off somewhere sunny and you end
up staying in the UK or something how how do you think
(0.8)
AL:
I’m [not planning] to s:tick around in the UK myself [xxx xxx xxx xxx xxxx xxx
INT:
[xxx]
[>but I mean you may< end
AL:
xxx xxx xxx xxx xxx xxx xxx xxx xxx]
INT:
up in different different parts of the world] or something like that=
AL:
=yeah the er th:e world is a small part
(0.2)
AL:
w- place now
(0.2)
INT:
˚m[:˚]
AL:
[u]::m
(0.4)
AL:
you know there’s always the internet there’s pho:nes I mean
(1.6)
AL:
he’s in Edinburgh which is as far away as it could be er um
(0.2)
AL:
Australia
(0.2)
INT:
yea[h]
AL:
[I] mean=
INT:
=yeah
(0.8)
AL:
so >I mean<
(0.3)
AL:
whilst I was living in Austria (.) u:m
(0.4)
AL:
my cousins and ↑I
(0.2)
AL:
quite close and they moved away sort of
(1.1)
AL:
˚.hh˚ (.) car distance you needed to travel so we didn’t see each other as oft but we
could have done (.) and when I moved away they what they said was
(1.1)
AL:
we could have seen each so often
(0.7)
AL:
but we didn’t and now that the:
(0.2)
AL:
I’m living so far away they want to see me
(0.3)
AL:
so
(0.7)
AL:
distance doesn’t really matter it’s just the effort you put into it I reckon
(0.7)
INT:
yeah
(0.6)
INT:
okay
(2.6)
INT:
u::m
(2.6)
INT:
tsk so this this chap you met at school
(0.2)
INT:
>did you I mean what were your other< school friends like
(1.5)
AL:
some were a pain
(0.2)
AL:
hh[h]
INT:
[h]a ha ha ha ha=
AL:
=.hhh
(0.3)
AL:
u:m
(0.2)
AL:
I haven’t had a easy life with friends I’ve always been sort of u:m (.) bullied I
never knew u:m
(0.4)
AL:
tsk
(0.9)
AL:
some of my friends: yr: took ad got taken advantage of I’m quite a
(0.6)
AL:
a nice guy you know does everything for everyone doesn’t day no: a:nd people
took advantage of me so=
INT:
=yeah
AL:
I sort of learnt the hard way
(0.2)
AL:
u:m so there are a few
(0.2)
AL:
folks like that but
(0.8)
AL:
xxx (.) I always got on tried to get on with everyone
(0.3)
AL:
u::m
(1.5)
AL:
I
(0.2)
AL:
can’t dis- I I
(0.3)
AL:
fail to dislike people in a way there’s only very rare people that I dislike and that’s
for a (.) very good reason you know they
(0.7)
AL:
if they upset my family or something <and>=
INT:
=yeah
(0.7)
AL:
that’s it (.) but otherwise
(1.2)
AL:
I get on well with people in general
(1.5)
INT:
wha (.) what sort of um
(1.0)
INT:
things do you mean by: you were taken advantage of
(1.8)
AL:
u::m hhh
(1.2)
AL:
just helping them in all sots of er
(0.2)
AL:
work u:m
(0.8)
AL:
>th th they’d< hang around with me when it suited them
(0.5)
INT:
m[:]
AL:
[n] then just drop me like
(0.7)
AL:
I was nothing er:u:m
(0.6)
AL:
n the next thing they’d be
(0.2)
AL:
bullying me teasing me and
(0.8)
AL:
I wouldn’t know why I was:
(0.5)
AL:
in place u::m
(2.0)
AL:
but
(0.7)
AL:
na:h
(0.4)
INT:
>and yet and yet<
(0.2)
INT:
>these were people who that you sort of< brought up in the context of friends as
[well so that] was
AL:
[<yeah>]
(1.2)
AL:
quite confusing sometime[s]
INT:
[y]eah
(0.5)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(2.8)
AL:
˚n: but˚
(0.6)
AL:
I don’t know it’s
(1.5)
AL:
been
(1.6)
AL:
with them with the my friends that I’ve got now w is we’ve got a sort of a banter
going on you know and we can say anything to each other and (.) which
(0.4)
AL:
if:: we say to another person could be quite hurtful n quite shocking n
(0.5)
AL:
people see it xxx xxx
(0.7)
AL:
yeah how can you let him talk to you like that n I go ˚puh˚ I give him as good
back and we know that it’s only jo:king and
(0.6)
AL:
there’s this girl in university and honestly she can say anything to me and I’d just
laugh it off because I know she’s joking (.) you know if you say it in one way
words may hurt but if only if you let them and if you mean it in a hurtful way
(0.9)
AL:
so
(.)
INT:
but she doesn’t
(.)
AL:
no
(0.5)
INT:
what sort of things I mean
(1.6)
AL:
oh crumby stu- er stupid things n you know c quite insulting things xxx xxx xxx how stupid I am and xxx and rubbish at this and this that and th- I don’t know it’s just
(1.3)
AL:
just petty things sometimes that get to people: ˚and it’s just˚
(0.7)
AL:
˚xx˚=
INT:
=xxx in the context of the friendship [it just]
AL:
[oh yeah]
(1.0)
AL:
just any[thing really]
INT:
[just forms] part of=
AL:
=<yeah>=
INT:
=how you speak to each other=
AL:
=exactly x[xx]
INT:
[ye]ah
(0.3)
INT:
okay
(0.9)
INT:
u:m:
(1.7)
INT:
↑how many of your other school friends are you still in touch with
(2.8)
AL:
hhh
(1.9)
AL:
u:m
(4.7)
AL:
xxx xxx quite a few xxx (.) go out every so often u:m just mean
(0.7)
AL:
th:ree of them majorly
(0.8)
AL:
which is my best fr- er
(0.4)
AL:
three friends u:[m]
INT:
[˚hm] m˚
(1.1)
AL:
one’s
(0.4)
AL:
sort of whenever he had a girlfriend he always s:tuck to his girlfriend sort of (.)
distanced himself from the group
(0.5)
AL:
so
(0.2)
AL:
<him I see every so often>
(0.7)
INT:
when he’s single
(0.7)
AL:
u[:m no no] this one I: think that’s gonna last for some r reason (.) u:m he’s been
INT:
[ha ha ha ha]
with her for: a few years now u:m
(1.1)
AL:
um when I [go]
INT:
[and you] still see him he’s not sort of isolated [xxx]
AL:
[he’s] not ↓isolated [no but]
INT:
[okay]
AL:
I s still see him every so often u:m
(0.5)
AL:
over facebook I like to sort of give people a a line every so often
(0.3)
AL:
u:m
(0.4)
AL:
I tend to like to keep in touch with people but I’m rubbish at it
(.)
INT:
hhh hhh ha ha h[a]
AL:
[I] intend to say I will phone (.) will do that I mean I phoned up
erm a friend .hhh that I went to school with a:nd er she said oh we need to keep in
contact more often yeah yeah .hhh that was January
(0.3)
AL:
h[hh] .hhh haven’t heard from her since
INT:
[ah]
(0.3)
INT:
so in four months that’s: (.) not brilliant=
AL:
=no [hhh]
INT:
[ha ha] ha ha ha (.) okay
(0.2)
AL:
.hhh
(0.8)
INT:
and there must be I mean there must have been other friends that you’ve sort of
drifted away from as well?
(.)
AL:
y:eah u:m
(0.3)
AL:
I mean obviously having lived in Austria for fifteen year fourteen fifteen years I
[had e]:r I’ve had
INT:
[˚m: ˚]
(0.5)
AL:
a few friends out there .hhh I mean one of them she:
(0.4)
AL:
funny enough (.) when she wa:::s: (.) ˚b˚
(0.2)
AL:
we’d just finished junior school so we were eleven she moved to ↑England a
Filipino girl her family moved to London
(0.5)
INT:
˚okay˚
(0.3)
AL:
a:n:d
(0.2)
AL:
we (.) kept in contact ˚b˚ by letters and that sort of er when I moved to England (.)
I met her once xxx that was five years later
(0.9)
AL:
u::m (.) n we met <once:>
(0.2)
AL:
<and>
(0.7)
AL:
she now has moved to the Philippines to do a dentistry course and I haven’t seen
her
(0.3)
AL:
in ten years and we almost lost conk tact (.) totally
(0.2)
INT:
˚m:˚=
AL:
=as in I used to send letters to: her family and everything and never got anything
back <and then> weirdly enough I’d find her online somewhere and think
(0.3)
AL:
FANTASTIC she’s there
(0.5)
AL:
and then: lose contact again with her and then out of the blue <she says> oh hello
I’m now on facebook as well
(1.1)
AL:
so u::m that’s one friend I sort of
(0.5)
AL:
on and off contact
(0.2)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(0.3)
AL:
u:m my best friend from junior school I used to be in contact with but
(1.0)
AL:
n I know I can find him on the internet in their newsletters because he’s er um um
(.) ↑a weight lifter for Austria
(0.9)
AL:
u:::m but
(0.7)
AL:
˚ps˚ I’ve lost total contact with him I’ve got his number
(0.3)
AL:
so I can try and pho:ne his family when I’m over
(1.1)
AL:
but u::m there’s one best friend he’s not internet (.) conne- he doesn’t like the idea
of internet (.) and stuff like he’s a bit backwards=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha ha=
AL:
=er::um (.) him I phone every time I’m in I’m in Austria and try (.) and meet up at
least once ˚s:o˚
(0.5)
INT:
do you go back to Austria quite a bit?
(0.2)
AL:
u:::m I
(0.5)
AL:
used
(0.5)
AL:
it’s more difficult now since my dad died last year
(0.7)
AL:
u:::m
(1.4)
AL:
but u:::m I stayed at I I’m going back in two weeks time
(0.8)
INT:
˚okay˚=
AL:
=u:m staying at my grandparents out there so u:m
(0.5)
AL:
I’m
(0.5)
AL:
intending to go at least once (.) or twice a year used to go twice a year with my
dad (.) to visit my dad ah um .hhh
(0.2)
AL:
now
(0.8)
AL:
sort of once or twice a year
(0.7)
AL:
˚x[xx xx]x˚
INT:
[okay]
(0.4)
INT:
and you intend to: they’ll they’ll be friends that you: hook up with when you’re
over there?
AL:
um yeah I try to yeah (.) er::m
(0.2)
AL:
like my one mate and then (.) this one girl I can just drop a mail and
(0.2)
AL:
we try and meet up
(1.2)
AL:
˚s[o˚]
INT:
[a]nd will you will you see a lot of them when you’re over there or just like see
them once and that’[s it]
AL:
[ah] it’s quite difficult I always u:m when I went over
(1.1)
AL:
I would see my dad we had
(0.2)
AL:
f:ull programme you know every single family member wanted to see you from
the
(0.4)
AL:
<GREAT aunt to: the cousins to this I mean every single day> we were doing
something el[se]
INT:
[ye]ah
(0.7)
AL:
a:n:d
(0.4)
AL:
I’d just have to say whoa I want to see one of my friends and I’d have to put them
in an evening and one for a day and it’s
(0.8)
AL:
it’s quite mental but
(.)
INT:
˚m:˚
(0.2)
AL:
think this time I might have bit of a more of a relaxed time
(1.0)
AL:
˚n˚ be able to do what I want
(0.6)
INT:
yeah
(0.3)
INT:
.hhh so when you see when you see them and you haven’t seen them in a while
(0.7)
INT:
how d how does that
(0.4)
INT:
how does that pan out
(1.5)
AL:
just as if we’d seen us er each other
(0.8)
AL:
˚f˚ er a few days be↑fore=
INT:
=o[kay]
AL:
[it’s] just (.) absolutely normal just cat[ch up] on (.) er: .hhh (.) stuff because
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
AL:
the one guy
(0.3)
AL:
I really didn’t have any hardly have any contact with people in Austria friends and
we s sort of catch up (.) and things and gossip a:n:d
(0.5)
AL:
what’s going on an[d]
INT:
[˚m]:˚
(.)
AL:
I mean I think
(0.4)
AL:
because of
(1.3)
AL:
xxx I was gonna meet up with him ˚m˚
(0.7)
AL:
las:::t time I was over in Austria
(0.3)
AL:
˚which is a y:::ear now˚
(1.4)
AL:
˚xxx xxx and a half ago˚
(1.6)
AL:
a:nd w the day that we were meant to meet up his parents (.) a:h sent him
somewhere that he needed to go y so we he didn’t have time to meet up
(1.1)
AL:
u:::m
(0.8)
AL:
I was gonna be there
(0.3)
AL:
I was xxx xxx xxx with my dad
(0.5)
AL:
so
(0.9)
AL:
I was only there for a week and it was really really mad mayhem so (.) couldn’t
meet up with him then
(0.4)
AL:
did try to but um he d- again didn’t have t↑ime
(0.8)
AL:
and then I went over with: my then <girlfriend>
(0.4)
AL:
<and>
(0.3)
AL:
˚he didn’t have don’t know we didn’t have time˚ or something like that
(0.6)
AL:
kind of didn’t work out
(1.2)
AL:
so
(0.3)
AL:
I d-
(0.5)
AL:
I haven’t seen him for
(0.5)
AL:
nearly two years now so
(0.5)
AL:
definitely try- I need to phone him after my exams and say by the way I’m
coming over to Austria make some time for m(h)e=
INT:
=yeah
(.)
AL:
.hh[h]
INT:
[ye]ah
(0.4)
AL:
but once I went over and he’d organised a a mini class reunion for me
(0.4)
AL:
a[s a sur]prise which was really good
INT:
[oh xxx]
(1.1)
AL:
see er someone actually greeted me and I said
(0.4)
AL:
and knew my name I was like
(0.5)
AL:
who the hell are you
(0.4)
AL:
look look at him and he said you don’t recognise me do you and I said no
(0.5)
AL:
he said who he was and I was like flipping heck
(0.7)
AL:
I mean he he was a s- like this when I s- u::m
(0.4)
AL:
when he was standing in front of me the last time I saw him he was quite a brick
you know he he was [a good] body shape and a pain in the arse didn’t like [him]
INT:
[yeah]
[ha] INT:
ha ha ha ha ha [ha ha]
AL:
[he’d] totally changed he was (.) totally: laid [back and a nice] guy
INT:
[cough cough]
and I was like (.) wo:w
(0.3)
INT:
yah
(0.4)
INT:
yeah
(0.6)
AL:
so yeah
(0.2)
INT:
˚cool˚
(2.1)
INT:
so the people that you do lose contact with what
(0.7)
INT:
w:hat’s
(1.3)
INT:
what are the sort of things that
(0.5)
INT:
end up meaning that you lose contact with them
(0.6)
AL:
˚jus˚
(0.7)
AL:
l:iterally lack of ti:me
(0.3)
AL:
[literal]ly u::m
INT:
[˚m: ˚]
(1.5)
AL:
I’ve literally only fallen out with one friend
(0.3)
AL:
before and that was (.) because of he was being so childish and so stupid it was on
his accord (.) Man United beat e:r Southampton which meant them being
relegated
(0.9)
AL:
a:::nd then the Monday afterwards
(0.6)
AL:
every day in the common room I asked every Monday I’d say oh how was your
weekend
(0.7)
AL:
and he’d just put his head down and someone j- just looked at me and shook his
head and I thought sh:i:t ha .hhh
(0.5)
AL:
refused to s- talk s- sit next to me refused to talk to me (.) absolutely (.) haven’t
spoken to him since
(1.9)
AL:
he wouldn’t accept any apologies from me nothing
(0.3)
AL:
this is we’re talking about an eighteen year o:ld
(0.3)
AL:
male person who
(1.7)
AL:
okay I cramped his pri:de a bit
(1.8)
INT:
hhh hhh
(0.2)
AL:
so puh
(0.7)
INT:
oh dear
(.)
AL:
m
(2.1)
INT:
so when you came to university
(0.7)
INT:
how did you go about meeking meeting
(0.3)
INT:
like new friends xxx
(0.4)
AL:
erm=
INT:
=>or did you have any< friends (.) from school that came to
u[niversity with you?]
AL:
[n::::]o=
INT:
=so [you it] was like completely new blank slate sort of thing
AL:
[no]
(.)
AL:
blank slate totally u::m well on the first day I walked in (.) u::m I was standing
there in front of the room I was like hhh .hhh ˚cra:p˚ hhh bit
(0.4)
AL:
new
(0.2)
AL:
<place new people>
(1.4)
AL:
and this guy
(0.6)
AL:
cockney
(0.3)
AL:
guy u:m he started talking to me and
(0.5)
AL:
˚s˚
(0.7)
AL:
hhhf someone’s talking to me (.) great=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha ha=
AL:
=and we was sat in the same room and he got taken out because he was in a um
different tutor group in the end and I had to stay in the room and I sat next to this
girl
(0.7)
AL:
u::m
(0.3)
AL:
and
(0.6)
AL:
who: started talking to me and sh- er we’d realised that we’d been (.) at
(0.3)
AL:
a ↑party together once
(1.1)
AL:
and had a er mutual ↑friend
(0.8)
INT:
˚okay˚=
AL:
=so u:m she had a few friends that were with her so (.) the lot of us went to the
um
(1.4)
AL:
ah what’s it called at the fair the the freshers’ fair
(.)
INT:
˚hm [m˚]
AL:
[˚x]x˚
(0.2)
AL:
where I totally lost them
(0.6)
AL:
xxx so I went home and er:m
(1.3)
AL:
I sort of: sat next to them and then this Irish girl that one u:m that takes the
mickey out of me she s:at there by us as well and >sort of< .hhh
(0.7)
AL:
went down to the er The Excelsior pub a:n:d sort of
(0.3)
AL:
welded together through booze
(.)
INT:
hhh hhh hhh
(.)
AL:
always works fantastically I think we were (.) the first time we all went there is
like
(0.5)
AL:
fiftee:n of us
(0.6)
AL:
a:nd just having a few drinks getting to know each other and
(.)
INT:
ye[ah]
AL:
[you] know
(1.0)
AL:
sort of still that that group of we sort of speak every so often and say hi n
(0.2)
AL:
you know (.) we know each other so
(1.3)
AL:
it’s kinda cool
(0.4)
INT:
xx makes them sound like they they they haven’t ended up being your like close
friends here but [you still s]ort of=
AL:
[n:::]
AL:
=no I mean I can talk to them
(0.4)
AL:
whateve[r I want] about I mean
INT:
[˚m:˚]
(2.0)
AL:
>it’s<
(2.0)
AL:
the girl that u:m
(1.4)
AL:
xxx xxx I can (.) yeah we’ve got a sort of a close-ish friendship I reckon
(0.5)
AL:
u:m
(1.7)
AL:
I mean I’ve babysat for one of my mates who was the first guy his erm
girlfriend’s got a ↑kid
(0.6)
AL:
a:n:d
(1.0)
AL:
they’re um very picky about p- er choosing people who are babysitting [n s]o
INT:
[˚hm˚]
(0.4)
AL:
um I’ve been
(0.2)
AL:
I’ve babysat for them a couple of times so
(.)
INT:
okay
(0.3)
AL:
I suppose
(0.9)
INT:
there’s a certain level of t[rust in play there and yeah]
AL:
[<xx certain level yeah so>]
(1.2)
AL:
there’s some good friends there I mean
(0.7)
AL:
definitely since that with my dad xxx xxx I’ve note they’ve er there’s been quite a lot
of support there from them as well so
(0.3)
INT:
yeah I was wanting to ask about that you [say] (.) you were saying (.) you
AL:
[˚m: ˚]
mentioned your dad died last year=
AL:
=yeah he [died last] Augus[t]
INT:
[last year]
[s]o
(0.6)
AL:
so (.) u:m
(0.6)
AL:
it was quite a shock
(0.9)
AL:
because it wasn’t expected he took his own life
(0.6)
AL:
[a::nd] so
INT:
[˚okay˚]
(1.2)
AL:
I was like
(0.3)
AL:
whoa it was like two days before my brothers were meant to go and see him as
well
(0.3)
AL:
and um I was meant to introduce my girlfriend to him
(0.5)
AL:
well then girlfriend and um
(1.8)
AL:
yeah it was kind of a strange experience I mean I needed my friends:
(0.2)
AL:
quite a bit when I came back
(0.9)
AL:
u:::m
(1.4)
AL:
and (.) talk just talked to them and you know there’s u:m
0.6)
AL:
it’s a very difficult subject to talk about
(0.3)
INT:
m:=
AL:
=u::m
(0.7)
AL:
well not just personally I don’t mind talking about it it’s just
(0.7)
AL:
some people don’t like to:
(0.7)
AL:
listen to it
(0.3)
AL:
a:nd talk about it and I’ve been quite lucky with the people that I’ve met here in
university also my best friends
(0.9)
AL:
e:r I’ve had quite a bit of support from them
(1.3)
INT:
so they’ve been okay to talk about it?=
AL:
=they’ve been absolutely fantastic yeah one of my mates actually said to me u:m
(0.6)
AL:
in fact recently a few months ago he said oh
(0.2)
AL:
didn’t realise you er were okay to talk about it I said (.) pff no problem about it you know and he went oh fine you know
(0.5)
AL:
and then sort of
(0.5)
AL:
chatted about my dad
er to me because he’d met him once so
(0.9)
INT:
okay
(1.1)
AL:
so that’s
(0.8)
INT:
so
(0.3)
INT:
when your dad died that you say that was August so
(0.7)
INT:
u the university hadn’t
(0.2)
INT:
sort of:
(0.7)
AL:
uni hadn’t s[ta:rted]
INT:
[you hadn’t uni hadn’t] started then
(0.3)
AL:
u:m no well that (.) which meant that u:m I was I wasn’t clear on when I was
coming back to university and everything so u:m obviously I needed people to
sort stuff out for me
(0.3)
INT:
okay
(0.2)
AL:
u:m
(1.1)
AL:
like registration: an[d] u:m getting booklets and stuff like that
INT:
[yeah]
(0.5)
AL:
cos=
INT:
=˚m[::˚]
AL:
[u:]m I
(0.2)
AL:
c went back again (.) in September over ↑registration period s[o I’m] missing a
INT:
[yeah]
couple of days
(0.8)
AL:
u:::m
(0.9)
AL:
so (.) obviously you need people to um
(0.3)
INT:
˚m[:˚]
AL:
[t]ake a few (.) notes n stuff [like that]
INT:
[.hhh so] were you in touch with your university
friends
during the summer
(.)
AL:
yeah
(.)
INT:
you were okay so so so it wasn’t like um
(0.4)
INT:
you had to hunt them down or you didn’t sort of [see them for months]
AL:
[oh: no not at all] no I fr u::m
(0.5)
AL:
just give em a bell I mean when we got our results through I was sitting there er
with my mates having a few drinks an:d I got a phone call
(0.8)
AL:
and it was ah: we’re just oh yeah what did you get and stuff like that so (.) yeah
it’s not as if oh
(1.2)
AL:
six months five months of not
(0.2)
AL:
no contact some of them yeah but some of them no but
(0.9)
AL:
u::m yeah I mean
(0.2)
AL:
through the internet as well now and (.) stuff (.) yeah
(.)
INT:
˚m˚
(0.8)
INT:
you’ve talked about that quite a lot the internet and facebook and MSN and that
(0.6)
INT:
you: (.) obviously
(0.4)
AL:
I’m an addict hhh=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha h[a]
AL:
[hon]estly five minutes b[efore I walked] into this room I thought
I’ve got
INT:
[y y you]
(0.2)
AL:
ten minutes let’s f- log on and s[ee what’s going on]
INT:
[ha ha ha ha ha] ha ha=
AL:
=.hhh whilst I was doing my revision here’s my laptop with facebook running
here’s my book (.) n my notes
(.)
INT:
yeah
(0.2)
AL:
it’s
(0.2)
AL:
I don’t know it’s a way of life now it’s just
(1.2)
AL:
˚x˚
(0.6)
INT:
and are you mostly in touch with people the people that you know on line
(0.2)
INT:
are they mostly people that you also know
(0.5)
INT:
u:m face to face
(0.4)
AL:
most of them yeah
(0.2)
INT:
b[ut not] all of them
AL:
[xxx]
(0.4)
AL:
well
(0.4)
AL:
only because I’ve d- I I’m gonna meet all of them
(0.3)
AL:
of u:m[:]
INT:
[o]kay[ alright]
AL:
[>because xxx<] but they’re the people that I’m
(0.8)
AL:
I’m going to camp with and on the trek with so
(0.6)
AL:
most ah: yeah I know
(0.5)
AL:
yeah I kno[w them]
INT:
[but other than] that they’re all people that you kno:w=
AL:
=yeah
(0.3)
INT:
yeah from around or from school or from (.) uni or whatever=
AL:
=yeah
(.)
INT:
okay great
(0.4)
INT:
˚okay˚
(1.8)
INT:
u:m
(4.0)
INT:
the: friends you’ve made at uni
(2.4)
INT:
how long do you think
(0.4)
INT:
you’re gonna stay
(0.4)
INT:
mates with them
(1.0)
AL:
hhh
(0.3)
AL:
.hhh
(0.8)
AL:
I’d like think <forever> (.) but er nothing’s forever r nu:m
(1.5)
AL:
I think there’s a couple of people that I I think
(0.8)
AL:
I’d stay t- er in contact with quite a bit
(0.4)
INT:
˚m:˚
(0.2)
AL:
u:::m
(1.4)
AL:
but (.) in a way you they say before you go to university you’re gonna meet the friends that you gonna n- er: be friends with the rest of your life (.) u::m
(0.6)
AL:
I don’t think it’s as easy as that
(0.4)
AL:
u::m
(0.8)
AL:
but I’m sure that u:m
(0.4)
AL:
a couple of us are going to stay er in contact and every so often
(0.7)
AL:
meet up with each other
(0.8)
AL:
like to think that
(0.3)
AL:
˚but˚
(0.5)
INT:
so why those people and not
(0.6)
INT:
other friends
(2.0)
AL:
UM
(1.4)
AL:
because we’re closer
(0.2)
INT:
˚m[: ˚]
AL:
[u]:m
(0.8)
AL:
some of them are sort of
(1.2)
AL:
I don’t know (.) it’s just
(0.9)
AL:
I’d like to think everyone (.) would s ah keep in contact but it never really happens [I mean not sort of a]
INT:
[nah life doesn’t] work like th[at does it xx]
AL:
[it doesn’t no] xxx you
(0.2)
AL:
you get your own life and then you know
(0.9)
AL:
a month becomes a yea:r: and that becomes ten years and then all of a sudden
(1.5)
AL:
you sort of meet on the street and say hi oh we need to meet up some time and
then
(0.7)
AL:
you’re eighty years old thinking .hhh all those people I’ve known
(0.8)
INT:
yeah
(1.6)
INT:
˚okay˚
(2.4)
INT:
((coughs)) sorry I’ve got a bit of a cough
(0.9)
INT:
um
(0.8)
INT:
can you think of an example f:rom your own experience that
(0.2)
INT:
illustrates
(0.4)
INT:
what friendship means to you (.) personally
(0.9)
AL:
friendship
(0.5)
AL:
to me is the m- the second most important thing in my life I reckon
(0.3)
AL:
after my family
(0.6)
INT:
okay
(.)
AL:
u::m
(0.6)
AL:
as I said they’re (.) like my chosen family some of the people
(.)
AL:
that [I’ve]
INT:
[˚hm] m˚=
AL:
=become friends with and (.) I couldn’t live without them
(0.9)
AL:
u:m I’m quite a
(0.5)
AL:
sort er
(0.2)
AL:
I- s- sometimes quite a loner sometimes I like to just sit in my room and do
absolutely nothing just sit on my Playstation you know n
(0.2)
AL:
play some fifa or something like that and (.) psome spend a bit of time of my life
but u:m
(2.0)
AL:
I’d say friends I need them as well you know go out have a drink you know
(0.3)
AL:
when something shit happens you know just go out and you can actually sit there
and get <absolutely> wasted and just
(0.6)
AL:
talk to them n (.) it’s ju- and or just have some fun as well you kno[w]
INT:
[˚m]: ˚
(0.8)
AL:
you can talk to them about things you can’t exactly talk to your family about u:m
(0.3)
AL:
you can sw↑ear a lot more with your friend[s]
INT:
[h]a ha ha ha [˚ha ha˚]
AL:
[.hhh]
(.)
AL:
ahe:r:m
(1.2)
AL:
you know f:riends
(1.2)
AL:
I say are for life as well as u:m family so you know
(0.5)
AL:
they really are important to me
(2.8)
INT:
can you think of like one particular
(0.8)
INT:
example of
(0.7)
INT:
kindness ↓maybe like a an act of kindness by a frien:d
(2.5)
AL:
um
(2.4)
AL:
<hhh>
(0.5)
AL:
I’m sure there’s many hhh=
INT:
=ha ha ha h[a]
AL:
[.h]hh if my frien[ds see that I’m sorry xxx]
INT:
[xxx xxx xxx ha ha] ha ha[ ha]
AL:
[.hh]h
(0.3)
AL:
u::m
(1.2)
AL:
act of kindness
(2.8)
AL:
↑erm I suppose
(3.1)
AL:
my best mate
(1.4)
AL:
inviting me to go to his villa
(0.2)
AL:
and e:rm also in er his family inviting me to stay
(0.4)
AL:
at their place in America for two and a half months n
(0.4)
AL:
t-
(0.3)
AL:
I wasn’t even allowed to pay anything er:m
(0.6)
AL:
I [had to] battle to give them anything and
INT:
[˚okay˚]
(0.2)
AL:
sh- his mum actually did all my ironing and everything so ↑I guess you know .hhh
s:uch little things um
(3.5)
AL:
u:m
(0.3)
AL:
just popping round my best mate just ah said oh I’m in the er area
(1.4)
AL:
mind if I pop round and stuff it’s just litt↓le things really
(1.1)
AL:
u::m
(1.5)
AL:
tsk[ so]
INT:
[oh] m- o- oh my
(0.2)
AL:
that’s one thing u::m
(1.0)
AL:
I::
(0.7)
AL:
saw this <watch>
(0.9)
AL:
i:n a a shop (.) once and I ex- I exp- explained what it looked like I said it’s this pocket w- I don’t like wearing watches I don’t like wearing too much on my a[rm]s
INT:
[okay]
(0.5)
AL:
and u:m it was this pocket watch
(0.4)
AL:
u::m it was sort of like a ↑stopwatch kind a er style
(0.7)
AL:
a:n:d I explained what I saw to my mate xxx w really good but you know hhh I
haven’t got the money it’s eighty quid (.) pff I don’t like spending money
(1.8)
AL:
twenty-first birthday
(0.8)
AL:
xxx turned up and there it was he’d bought it me in America
(1.2)
INT:
o[h wow]
AL:
[I suppose] that that
i::s u::m
(1.2)
AL:
yeah it’s one of the best things I think a=
INT:
=so what did that (.) I mean what did that mean to you
(0.6)
AL:
everything cos I’d (.) also just lost my dad and I’d just been <wow>
(0.4)
AL:
you know
(0.3)
AL:
<that really just shows>
(0.3)
AL:
what a mate is he
(2.0)
AL:
he as I said he’s a pain in the arse=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha
(0.4)
INT:
and
(0.6)
AL:
very u::m
(0.6)
AL:
you know
(2.1)
AL:
he just says (.) what he thinks and everything and
(0.5)
AL:
but you know he never <shows>
(0.4)
AL:
too much how he cares
(0.9)
AL:
that just sort of symbolises our friendship I reckon
(1.6)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(1.2)
INT:
yeah I mean it (.) it
(0.5)
INT:
strikes me that it means that he had to
(1.5)
INT:
be paying attention in the first place
(0.4)
AL:
whi[ch he’s] pays lack of sometimes I [talk to] him and he er ask him something=
INT:
[and er]
[really?]
AL:
=and he says sorry were you talking to me=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha [ha ha]
AL:
[I was li]ke cheers mate I’ve just babbled on on a mono[logue xxx
INT:
[ha ha
AL:
here for the] last ten minutes .hhh
INT:
ha ha]
(0.5)
INT:
yeah
(0.8)
INT:
and then he had to remember it=
AL:
=m:
(0.2)
INT:
and then he had to
(1.4)
INT:
you know go to the effort of picking it out as well
(0.2)
AL:
yeah .hhh I mean as he I said oh wow I can’t believe you got me this and you
know remembered this he said well
(0.5)
AL:
I ↑wasn’t actually looking for anything like that
(0.2)
INT:
.hhh [hhh]
AL:
[I] just s:aw it and it was on display and it was the last one so I bought it I
said .hhh pff (.) big deal kind of thing but
(0.7)
AL:
it meant a lot to me
(.)
INT:
yeah
(1.4)
INT:
do you still have that? obviously?
(0.3)
AL:
y:eah: (.) I think it’s on the floor in between my be(h)d and my [desk at the
INT:
[ha ha
AL:
mo(h)me(h)n]t
INT:
ha ha ha]
(0.3)
AL:
.hhh
(0.2)
INT:
yeah you said you weren’t so tidy [ha] ha ha [ha] ha
AL:
[hhh] [˚no˚]
(0.3)
AL:
.hhh
(0.5)
INT:
˚okay˚
(0.8)
INT:
↑you ever been let down by a friend?
(0.8)
AL:
quite a few times hhh er::m
(0.8)
AL:
sometimes I thought they were a friend and they ended up being just
(1.1)
AL:
people you couldn’t trust
(1.4)
AL:
a::nd just lied to you or u:m you know in the end
(0.9)
AL:
bullied you <and> you kno[w xxx]
INT:
[>could you give me an<] example?
(0.6)
AL:
ah[(h)]
INT:
[like w]
(0.2)
INT:
a particular person
(0.2)
AL:
a particular person u::m
(2.2)
AL:
yeah that guy that u:m
(3.2)
AL:
n- ended up being really skinny and quite nice in the end aw after I’d seen him not
seen him for ages er
(0.5)
AL:
we used to be really good friends ind um high school first couple of years and
then he j sort of <turned> I mean ˚
he he I think˚ he had a mental problem I think
(0.4)
AL:
because
(0.2)
AL:
he used to make ↑swor:ds out of u:m
(0.5)
AL:
pieces of metal and everything and used to have sword fights and
(0.6)
AL:
NOT JUST PLAYFUL (.) HE WOULD ACTUALLY WHACK YOU
(0.5)
AL:
and he went absolutely ape and then whenever someone else w but just the two of
us were fine
(0.5)
AL:
and then some a third party came to it and (.) he just turned on me and just <took
the mick out of me and made me feel this big>
(0.8)
AL:
<and>
(1.9)
AL:
foolishly kept crawling back but
(0.3)
AL:
in the end [I just though]t sod you
INT:
[˚m:˚]
(0.6)
INT:
tsk how long did that go on for?
(0.9)
AL:
hhh few months not a yea:r something like that
(3.0)
INT:
so why did you
(1.8)
INT:
why did you keep going back do you think
(0.7)
AL:
because I’m
(0.2)
AL:
a forgiving idiot tha[t u::]:m
INT:
[ha ha ha ha]
(1.8)
AL:
I oh just see the best in people constantly you know
(2.2)
AL:
I don’t know f::ool
(1.1)
AL:
˚js˚
(0.2)
AL:
always had friends n I hated u::m (.) seeing bad things in them and thinking that
they’re horrib[le n stu]ff like that n
INT:
[˚m:˚]
(2.6)
AL:
suppose I was used to it
(2.1)
AL:
always had someone pick on me so
(0.7)
INT:
has it been something
sort of fairly
(0.3)
INT:
I don’t know constant [for you xxx]
AL:
[yeah (.) from k]indergarten age up there was always the
group in the er:
(0.3)
AL:
((coughs)) in the um
(1.6)
AL:
play area where I used to live in Austria that
(0.9)
AL:
individually some people were great (.) get the group together a:nd I was the hunted
(1.6)
AL:
so
(2.1)
AL:
this this one kid he was always j- jealous of me because I used to be quite friendly and you know greet his mum n everything and she used to turn round and why can’t you be like that .hhh
(0.3)
AL:
YEAH not the GREATEST THING TO SAY TO YOUR SON I [RECKON]
INT:
[<yea]h>
(0.8)
INT:
not clever tactica[lly]
AL:
[n]:o
=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha ha
(3.2)
INT:
so this
(2.0)
INT:
this um
(0.7)
INT:
this friend you were talking about who:
(1.1)
INT:
˚u:m˚
(0.9)
INT:
˚who˚ beat you with s:(h)words
(.)
AL:
h[hh]
INT:
[ha ha] (h)it
(0.2)
AL:
.h[hh]
INT:
[d(h)oes] s(h)ound (.)[ does] sound f(h)airly hecti[c]
AL:
[hhh]
[h]hh .hh[h]
INT:
[.h]hh ((coughs))
(1.4)
INT:
what I mean what sort of impact does that have on you
(1.1)
AL:
tsk
(0.8)
AL:
pff
(0.2)
AL:
I (.) reckon it’s character building it sort of gives you life u::m experience <and>
you know
(1.6)
AL:
my brother’s now going through a similar experience at this very moment you know he had this best friend and all of a sudden there was a third person that who’s absolute
(1.0)
AL:
erh >don’t know he’s a<
(0.6)
AL:
year seven and he thinks he:’s the world and
(0.7)
AL:
whereas my brother was friends just friends with him (.) he changed he was rude
he li:ed he was totally character changing
(0.3)
INT:
˚m:˚
(0.2)
AL:
and you see he got away and he became quite nice and he spent a lot of time with
this other guy and who’s now s (.) the three of them then (.) sort of the two of
them (.) got friends with
(0.2)
AL:
again with that (.) guy
(0.9)
AL:
a:n:d
(0.4)
AL:
it escalated turned on my brother
(1.8)
AL:
and
(0.5)
AL:
I just saw my lif::e
(0.3)
AL:
rolling through
(0.6)
AL:
past me again through my brother
(0.2)
INT:
˚m:˚
(.)
INT:
And it’s like
(1.2)
AL:
it’s sort of c- I can now I’ve
(0.5)
AL:
gained the experience I can take him aside and say
(0.7)
AL:
this is right this is wrong
(0.2)
AL:
you know
(0.4)
AL:
can help him
(1.0)
AL:
u:m again I can
(0.3)
AL:
myself then now see
(1.1)
AL:
who is actually my friend
(0.9)
AL:
ah:::r
(0.7)
AL:
I suppose I’ve ˚n˚ I’ll never lea:rn properly
(0.2)
INT:
˚h[a ha˚]
AL:
[nev]er do
(0.7)
AL:
u::m
(0.3)
AL:
cos I always see the best in people (.) but
(0.5)
AL:
you know
(0.2)
AL:
it’s
(0.5)
AL:
the trust thing I think is
(0.4)
AL:
a bit of a high standard I don’t trust people as soon as I meet them
(0.7)
AL:
I need to gai- I need to gain their tru[st xxx xxx]
INT:
[whereas you used t]o
(0.2)
AL:
ye[ah]
INT:
[used to] just trust them
(2.6)
INT:
so do you
(0.5)
INT:
if you think about those experiences now and you think about the friends that
you’ve currently got
(0.4)
INT:
are there sort of people who you think might sort of
(0.7)
INT:
be like that if if given the chance?
(1.7)
AL:
˚I hope not˚
(0.2)
AL:
no: hhh .hhh NO definitely not
(1.1)
AL:
my best mate no the the group that we’ve got now na:h
(0.7)
AL:
definitely not
(0.8)
INT:
so how’s the group the you got n:ow different from
(0.3)
INT:
the groups of friends you’ve had in the past
(1.1)
AL:
u:m=
INT:
=cos you talk about them in quite a different way=
AL:
=yeah (.) I mean (.) d- I
(1.1)
AL:
I left Austria when I was fourteen so
(1.1)
AL:
the groups of chi:ldhood friends there’s di[fferent] friends whereas you know
INT:
[˚m:˚]
(2.5)
AL:
I needed I was desperate fru- ha I needed a group of friends in England so it it got
a lot tighter er .hhh we spent near enough ev- we’d spent every day together n
everything and the weekends we spent together we went to the cinemas we er
went to the .hhh as we got old enough well
(0.9)
AL:
<theoretically> in my mi:nd I was old enough because [I was brought] up
INT:
[˚ha ha ha˚]
(.0.2)
AL:
l[iving in ↑Austria]
INT:
[no matter what the law] says ha [ha ha ha ha]
AL:
[you can] drink from the age of sixteen in
Austria=
INT:
=okay=
AL:
=↑no matter what (.)˚th˚ the (.) border changes I was allowe[d]
INT:
[h]a ha ha ha
(.)
AL:
mummy ↑said so (.) I can
(0.3)
INT:
((coug[hs))]
AL:
[u:m] so
(1.7)
AL:
that was fine xxx so I needed a sort of tighter group n that we just
(0.5)
AL:
knitted up tightly
(1.5)
INT:
tsk but some of the pe- some of the friends from school were
(0.5)
INT:
w:ere people who then let you down
(0.4)
INT:
afterwards?
(0.6)
AL:
i[n e]r
INT:
[˚xx˚]
(0.3)
AL:
in Austria yeah=
INT:
=>oh the oh so [so that was sort of stuff that happened xxx< okay alright right]
AL:
[th th th th that was in Austria that they were in Austria] the
English I’ve not really had too much problems in England with friends
(0.2)
INT:
okay
(.)
AL:
as too much
(1.3)
INT:
alright
(0.4)
AL:
I mean I s::till see o:ld school friends walking about that I’m not really in contact
with and it’s sort of alright there and this that and the other and just
(0.5)
AL:
a quick hello and
(0.3)
INT:
yeah
(.)
AL:
˚so˚
(0.4)
AL:
˚never really have any problems˚
(0.6)
INT:
˚okay˚
(3.9)
INT:
↑so
(2.1)
INT:
<what>
(1.5)
INT:
<does> friendship
(0.6)
INT:
actually mean to you
(1.0)
AL:
trust
(0.9)
INT:
˚yeah?˚
(0.5)
AL:
u::m
(0.5)
AL:
tsk you’ve gotta r- be able to rely
(0.9)
AL:
on them
(1.5)
AL:
er::
(1.5)
AL:
I suppose
(1.7)
AL:
you’ve gotta (.) you
(0.3)
AL:
yourself aren’t (.) just relying on them you need to be able to say
(0.2)
AL:
>you know<
(0.7)
AL:
this is my best mate you’ve gotta turn round and say .hhh
(0.4)
AL:
he’s the same he (.) I’m the same to him so
(0.9)
AL:
you’ve gotta be able to
(0.9)
AL:
say
(0.6)
AL:
I will (.) take
(0.4)
AL:
>responsibility< I will not tell anyone about this I will be there for them (.) when
they need me
(1.1)
AL:
you(h) so i- it’s it’s a two way thing
(0.2)
AL:
you know
(0.8)
AL:
if he
(0.2)
AL:
needs you
(0.6)
AL:
vice versa
(1.3)
AL:
so: yeah it’s (.) I think the main aspect is trust
(3.1)
INT:
okay
(0.8)
INT:
˚cool˚
(4.3)
INT:
gonna have a quick go through and make sure we haven’t mi[ssed anything] out
AL:
[˚xxx xx˚]
that I was supposed to be looking at
(3.6)
INT:
um
(4.1)
INT:
tsk I think we’ve covered
(0.2)
INT:
˚er˚ I think we’ve covered the
(0.5)
INT:
the main points
(.)
AL:
m[:]
INT: [IS] THERE ANY IS THERE ANYTHING that
(0.2)
INT:
that (.) erm
(0.2)
INT:
that’s occurred to you? ˚while we’ve been talking˚
(0.6)
AL:
e[rh]
INT:
[abou]t about friends and friendships xxx stories of friendship n what have you
that that we’ve not really
(0.5)
AL:
tsk(.h) I suppose
(1.4)
AL:
as I’ve been speaking you
(0.5)
AL:
it’s as you said you know you’ve had friends here and there you can always say
friends come and go: but you know
(0.5)
AL:
the ones that are really important to you
(0.5)
AL:
you should always try to kee:p
(0.2)
AL:
in touch with n tre- keep around you
(.)
INT:
˚m:˚
(.)
AL:
cos you never know
(1.0)
AL:
when you next need them
(0.3)
AL:
because I reckon
(0.9)
AL:
at the end of your life
(1.9)
AL:
you might have your kids n everything
(0.3)
AL:
˚but˚ you know
(2.4)
AL:
you’ve er in the same b- you you’ve never got someone really
(0.2)
AL:
else in the same boat
(0.8)
AL:
that’s sort of say
(.)
INT:
((coughs))
(0.5)
AL:
because
(0.4)
AL:
you there’s always a a (.) a difference yes people have gone through the same
things but once you’ve sort of lost your parents your your older generation n
you’re the oldest generation in the family
(0.4)
INT:
˚hm m˚
(0.5)
AL:
you haven’t really got anything to compa:re (.) with
(0.5)
AL:
you always you ha- can’t like now you I (.) say to my mother AH: this that n the
other they other I hate life this is CRAP .hhh n she said oh I went though this you
know it’s difficult I guess (.) this that n the other you know .hhh she’s there to
give you advice and you know
(0.2)
AL:
talk through her life experience n
(0.2)
AL:
any other er adult can do that
(0.7)
AL:
but
(0.3)
AL:
once you’ve lost that (.) I reckon you need your friends because you going through the same things at the same time
(1.0)
AL:
cos
(0.8)
AL:
yeah you speak to w- I speak to my grandparents about l:ife n stuff like that but
(0.2)
AL:
˚when I’m that age I don’t think I’d remember that much˚
(0.4)
AL:
it’s [difficult] enough now to remember th- just [as much]
INT:
[hhh]
[ha ha ha]
(0.4)
AL:
er::m
(0.7)
AL:
so y- I reckon you nee:d them throughout your whole life
(0.7)
AL:
and if you stick with someone that’s been throu:gh y-
(0.6)
AL:
with you for your whole life
(1.5)
AL:
you can sit back and say oh:: just had a laugh about (.) y- when you were
younger what you got up to n you know just see life pass n (.) you know help each
other through the the later stages of life as well you know
(1.3)
INT:
.hhh so the friends
(0.2)
INT:
that
(0.3)
INT:
say you went to school with over here
(0.2)
AL:
m:
(.)
AL:
u:m
(0.7)
INT:
even if I mean they haven’t ended up at the sam:e uni as you or anything
[like that] but they’re obviously they’re still at uni most of them I gue[ss?]
AL:
[m:]
[y]eah
(0.5)
INT:
u:m so you’re still sharing that sort of experience=
AL:
=m:
(0.5)
INT:
are there ↑friends
(0.3)
INT:
that you had at school who haven’t
(0.3)
INT:
gone on to uni they’ve like gone on to job[s or something] like th[at?]
AL:
[.hhh]
[u:]:m (.) one
friend er she’s um
(1.4)
AL:
she’s j- Open University she went on to work
(0.2)
AL:
u:m
(0.6)
AL:
and then realised that she
(0.3)
AL:
could (.) do needed >a a< a degree to help her along and so
(1.0)
AL:
she:’s u:m
(0.6)
AL:
done an Open n- University degree
(0.3)
AL:
and (.) working at the same time
(.)
INT:
okay
(0.9)
AL:
u::m[:]
INT:
[so] her life’s quite different to the yours [sort] of a full time student=
AL:
[y:eah]
AL:
=yeah she she moved over to Liverpool not long after ↑I did and
(0.5)
AL:
weirdly enough we only er meet up really on the ↑Wirral when everyone’s round we’re so cl- we’re literally like ten minutes away from each other and (.) do not see each other .hhh
(.)
INT:
yea[h]
AL:
[e]:r:m but
(1.4)
AL:
yeah u:m
(1.3)
AL:
as in some of the older ones (.) have got jobs
(0.3)
AL:
they um
(0.2)
AL:
˚never went to some of them didn’t go to university˚ they’re now starting
university because they’re realising that
(0.3)
AL:
they can’t do anything else
(0.3)
AL:
w- without a degree
(0.8)
AL:
u:::m
(0.9)
AL:
but
(0.9)
AL:
I think the majority of them did go to university because
(0.3)
AL:
u:m ((SCHOOL NAME)) was a grammar school who sort of tried to um
(0.6)
AL:
enforce university on you
(1.2)
AL:
u::m
(1.9)
AL:
˚n I think yeah my friends all went˚
(0.9)
AL:
˚my close friends anyway˚
(0.4)
INT:
˚m˚
(0.5)
INT:
˚okay˚
(0.6)
INT:
((sniff)) yeah I was I was sort of
(0.7)
INT:
I guess I was fishing a bit
(0.3)
INT:
u:m to see if
(1.1)
INT:
maybe that friends that
(0.3)
INT:
didn’t end up in the same sort of situation tended to be: the ones that you sort of
tended to drift away from more but
(0.4)
INT:
doesn’t seem[ to] be really=
AL:
[well]
AL:
=there is one girl who never went to university and I got on really well in school n
we always said s I haven’t seen her since school n we’ve always said oh oh we
need to do
(0.2)
AL:
meet meet need to meet up meet u[p and] er:
INT:
[˚m::˚]
(0.7)
AL:
she actually got married last August
(1.4)
AL:
˚she’s˚ younger than ↑me
(0.2)
AL:
but (.) thinking about it my mum was what twenty-one when she married as well
and but
(.)
INT:
ye[ah]
AL:
[I] I can’t imagine me getting [married I (.)] I’m still a ki:d inside
INT:
[ha ha ha]
(0.7)
AL:
I can’t get (.) I no way e:r:m but yeah she’d been with the same guy since she was
what f::ift↑een=
INT:
=really
(0.4)
AL:
and u:m yeah I got invited to the wedding
(0.4)
INT:
˚m:˚=
AL:
=obviously I
(0.2)
AL:
couldn’t go b- being in in August and my dad al- and=
INT:
=alright okay
(0.4)
AL:
I still haven’t met her she’s moved in xxx xxx o:h we need to meet up n oh come
round to my place ˚and it’s like no˚
(0.3)
AL:
˚<and> no can’t af-˚
(0.2)
AL:
I haven’t emailed her for ages and then there’s one girl who’s a deejay
(1.0)
AL:
and she jets around the world and she’s busy (.) you know
(0.4)
AL:
in her business and earning her own money and her own life xxx I see her every
so often
(0.3)
AL:
˚but˚
(0.2)
INT:
˚m:˚
(0.9)
AL:
yeah=
INT:
=is it
(0.4)
INT:
is it sort of ↑harder when you see her because:
(0.2)
INT:
your lives are so ↑different or does it not matter=
AL:
=˚it doesn’t matter˚ [see I] I:
INT:
[it doesn’t m]
(0.7)
AL:
I always describe myself as a (.) bit of Blu <Tack> ˚you know I mould myself
into this˚ right situations I mould myself in with people n you know I’m easy
going you know
(0.2)
AL:
you want a boozy group of friends well I can a be a boozy person don’t expect (.)
me to drink
(0.4)
AL:
too many spirits n stuff but I’ll stick with the beer and I’ll be rowdy and er boozy
or
(0.3)
AL:
I can be the person who sits lies back a:nd relaxes watches a film or plays some
Playstation as well I
(1.1)
AL:
I don’t mi:nd as long as I hang around with some people and they’re nice enough
to hang around with
(1.2)
INT:
so depending on the circumstances you:
(0.8)
INT:
almost like a different person?
(0.4)
AL:
I wouldn’t say a different person I’m always me
(0.4)
INT:
so h[ow] what’s what’s the difference=
AL:
[u::m]
AL:
=the difference is that (.) u::m
(1.4)
AL:
the group situation is different as in
(0.7)
AL:
.hhh I suppose I am different once I’ve had a <drink>
(0.4)
AL:
u::m
(1.1)
AL:
but
(0.5)
AL:
u::m
(1.6)
AL:
no I:
(0.2)
AL:
I’m
(0.4)
AL:
I’m the sam:e me
(0.4)
AL:
I’m still u::m
(0.7)
AL:
it’s just
(0.8)
AL:
I’m okay in many situations I I
(0.3)
AL:
I enjo:y drinking as much as I enjoy relaxing I enjoy just ta:lking and (.) you
know different people diff different situation (.) different people like different
things and
(1.7)
AL:
I suppose I
(0.8)
AL:
could
(0.4)
AL:
meet a bunch of strangers who are set in their way who are looking for a room
mate and I say well
(0.7)
AL:
I just fit in
(0.2)
AL:
and I’d ju[st] go along with them (.) and have no problem with it
INT:
[˚m˚]
(0.9)
INT:
˚okay˚
(0.4)
INT:
.hhh you said you said something quite early on in the interview that I wanted that
I was interested in
(0.7)
INT:
about
(0.4)
INT:
friends being the family that you choose=
AL:
=m:
(1.0)
INT:
((coughs))
(0.6)
INT:
and I I like the idea of
(0.4)
INT:
of choice in friendship but I suspect it’s not all that
(0.6)
INT:
it’s not all that simple because then when you ta:lk about the f- the bunch of
friends you ended up with
(0.4)
INT:
um (.) they happened to be people who spoke to you: on the first day of university
and all that sort of stuff and it doesn’t see:m to me .hhh as much of a choice
(.)
AL:
.hhh well I(h) hhh
(0.2)
AL:
mean
(0.9)
AL:
I I could say you you
(1.2)
AL:
it’s fate who you meet (.) it’s (.) but you still choose if you want to be friends with
them
(.)
INT:
˚okay˚
(0.4)
AL:
u::m
(0.4)
AL:
I mean (.) u
(1.8)
AL:
yeah you I think you
(0.4)
AL:
you nee-
(0.2)
AL:
hhh
(1.0)
AL:
it i:s in a way you’re right
(0.3)
AL:
you
(0.4)
AL:
you meet a a load of people in your life I reckon
(0.9)
AL:
and
(0.2)
AL:
you:
(0.3)
AL:
either you choose I don’t know if it’s choo- er you choosing or it’s fate or
something
(0.3)
AL:
but u:m
(1.3)
AL:
>in a way<
(0.3)
AL:
you try n
(0.2)
AL:
find (.) the ones that are right for you
(0.6)
AL:
I mean this girl that I’ve just met on the internet I mean I’ve never got on as I said
so well with [someone] just
INT:
[˚m:˚]
(0.6)
AL:
by briefly knowing them
(0.7)
AL:
u::m
(0.3)
AL:
xxx
(0.4)
AL:
as if I know her inside out now (.) u::m
(0.9)
AL:
but
(0.3)
AL:
I said to her it’s <total fate that we’ve met>
(0.6)
AL:
absolute >there was< if my dad had not died
(0.7)
AL:
I would not have met this girl cos I wouldn’t have ha:d they money to go
(0.5)
AL:
on this trek
(0.6)
AL:
u:m if my brother would have (.) decided to go on a trek with me
(0.8)
AL:
I wouldn’t have gone on the same tre:k as her
(0.5)
AL:
there’s so many coincidences I mean we nearly me:t each other at the s- at Camp
America orienteering she was in the same room
(0.3)
AL:
I was sitting at the back of the room and she was sitting a few rows in front of me
(1.0)
AL:
and we n- didn’t meet I mean
(0.8)
AL:
it’s s:trange how people (.) are in the same room
(.)
INT:
˚m:˚
(0.3)
AL:
don’t meet but end up weirdly knowing each other (.) somehow through the
internet
(1.4)
AL:
so:
(0.5)
AL:
in a way it’[s >fate] that I know her and I< know that
INT:
[yeah]
(0.5)
AL:
I’m ho:ping it means something really special a special friendship something that
(0.6)
AL:
we need to we’ll have to something’s gonna happen where
(0.6)
AL:
for some reason we need each other or something (.) I don’t know
(0.3)
INT:
˚m:˚
(1.0)
AL:
so
(2.1)
INT:
is there a possibility that something romantic could happen there?=
AL:
=a::h (.) no
(0.3)
AL:
I [thought] at first before I met her over the internet I mean she’s a she’s a really=
INT:
[˚no˚]
AL:
=good er I really get on with her she’s very similar she’s as clumsy as me she’s
[er xx .hhh] this that n the other we’re so similar except when it comes to eating=
INT:
[ha ha ha ha ha]
AL:
=she’s vegetarian and very very picky on what she eats she she’s a vegetarian that
doesn’t even eat salad
(1.0)
INT:
(h)okay=
AL:
=y:es very stra:nge u:m that’s the only difference I reckon er one of the only
differences and um
(0.6)
AL:
tsk she u::m
(0.8)
AL:
but
(0.4)
AL:
>and she’s a< good looking girl you know she’s got the two combinations to be
the perfect girlfriend I thought to myself (.) and I met her <and> instantly I knew
no
(0.7)
AL:
not the vibes she was giving me it was (.) ˚just˚
(0.3)
AL:
it wasn’t the:re there was nothing=
INT:
=it was just friends?=
AL:
=yea:h
(0.3)
INT:
˚okay˚
(1.4)
AL:
˚so˚
(0.9)
AL:
so
(0.5)
AL:
I mean (.) who knows what’s in the future but
(0.5)
INT:
yeah
(0.2)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(0.7)
INT:
˚I mean you’re gonna be˚
(0.2)
INT:
.hhh with her and a whole bunch of other people for quite a while on the trek so
(.)
AL:
yeah=
INT:
=˚xxx˚=
AL:
=there’s gonna be thirty [xxx xxx]
INT:
[↑what what d’]you erm
(3.7)
INT:
>↑what<
(1.7)
INT:
qualities in other people do you look for in your friends I mean
(0.3)
AL:
tsk .hhh
(1.0)
INT:
y[ou you talked] about trustworthyn[ess w w ] w w wha[t what] what else:=
AL:
[hhh]
[yeah tru]
[˚oh˚(h)]
AL:
=w[hat else]
INT:
[what else is] important to you
(0.9)
AL:
obvi- that they’re friendly
(0.6)
AL:
and nice u::m
(0.7)
AL:
that they accept me (.) hhh .hhh for who I am u:m
(1.6)
AL:
u:↑:m
(1.1)
AL:
˚>I guess<˚
(2.4)
AL:
˚i-˚
(0.2)
AL:
diff:
(0.6)
AL:
˚xx˚ enjoy certain interests you know they like s- you now like going for a
dri:nk u[:]:m
INT:
[˚m˚]
(3.3)
AL:
˚what other qualities˚
(0.3)
AL:
˚that’s awful˚ (.) hhh=
INT:
=ha ha ha ha=
AL:
=yeah I want my friends to be able to drink (.) be[ able to be trust]worthy u::m:
INT:
[ha ha ha ha ha]
(0.9)
AL:
.hh[h] I don’t know I don’t go out to think I want this to be my frie- er this [xxx xxx
INT:
[˚xx˚]
[no no I
AL:
friends u:m]
INT:
guess it’s a fairly sort of arti]ficial question but=
AL:
=u::m
(0.3)
AL:
I just when I meet someone I g-
(0.6)
AL:
>.hhh I g-<
(0.2)
AL:
similar interests I think reckon u:m=
INT:
=okay
(0.3)
AL:
because my best friend and I have a interest in travelling both of us I mean I got
tra- u:m I always liked the idea of travelling but once I went travelling round
Europe and America I just got (.) bitten by the travel bug and .hhh
(0.2)
AL:
I just want to spread my wings and see the whole world I mean I want to
(0.6)
AL:
taste and drink myself through it so
(1.8)
INT:
hh[h]
AL:
[I] don’t know it’s similar interests is always a good w-=
INT:
=yeah=
AL:
=one
(1.4)
INT:
˚okay˚
(2.0)
INT:
.hhh ↑alright well I think I’m done=
AL:
=m:
(0.4)
INT:
er I’ve sort of gone off script at the end but I don’t mind xxx ha ha .hhh so thanks
for that=
AL:
=no problem [at all]
INT:
[u:]m <I’ll>
(0.4)
INT:
stop the video and I’ve got one other thing for you
(6.0)
*END OF RECORDING*
PAGE
2
LOUISE
(2.2)
INT:
˚okay˚
(2.5)
INT:
right hhh
(0.2)
INT:
are those boots not incredibly hot in this weather?
(0.4)
LOU:
no: they’re too comfortable a ha ha ha
(1.0)
LOU:
I [live] in them now hhh he [he]
INT: [˚my˚]
[my] folks sent me a pair for uh my in-laws sent me a
pair from New Zealand=
LOU:
=yeah
(0.5)
INT:
um (.) sort of big sort of
(0.4)
INT:
bloke’s sized
(0.2)
INT:
Ugg boots
(0.6)
INT:
and they’re I mean they’re toastie but I couldn’t [wear] them now
LOU:
[yeah]
(.)
LOU:
could ya ha
(0.5)
LOU:
strange the weather outside today isn’t it
(0.3)
LOU:
s (.) really like
(2.2)
LOU:
hum[id n]
INT:
[not] liking it at [all yest]erday was great=
LOU:
[n:o]
LOU:
=it was beautiful yesterday [but]
INT:
[but] not liking this at all
(1.0)
INT:
so: ↑okay
(0.3)
INT:
what I’d like to do is just run this so:rt of like a conversation >I mean I’ve got<
(0.4)
INT:
I’ve got an interview schedule here which I’ll be going through
(3.1)
LOU:
right=
INT:
=u:m
(0.4)
INT:
so that’s that’s what I’m looking at=
LOU:
=yeah
(.)
INT:
um
(0.2)
INT:
just so as you know >you can have a look at it< if you like
(0.6)
INT:
u:m and it’s got sort of a bunch of questions but it’s more like
(0.3)
INT:
various areas that I want to ask questions about abou[t friendship and I’d] rather it
LOU:
[yeah that’s xxxx]
INT:
was more a conversation than a question and answer thing=
LOU:
=>yeah<
(0.2)
INT:
so I will: (.) go off on si:de track[s an]d whatever and I really I really don’t mind=
LOU:
[alright]
INT:
=that
(1.7)
INT:
um what’s good is if
(0.9)
INT:
if when we’re ta:lking you can actually sort of
(0.5)
INT:
come up with like examples like specific examples
(0.7)
INT:
like telling stories about you know with this friend this h[appened and all a]ll that
LOU:
[right (.) okay]
INT:
sort of stuff and that [makes] a it makes it more interesting and more like
LOU:
[hm ↑m]
INT:
personally relevant?=
LOU:
=yeah sure=
INT:
=and um:
(0.2)
INT:
and and also
(0.6)
INT:
sometimes it can really help with working with the data after[wards] so
LOU:
[yeah]
(0.8)
INT:
u:m:
(1.0)
INT:
tsk okay
(0.4)
INT:
all:: right
(1.2)
INT:
are you sitting comfortably? (said in a ‘comedy’ voice)
(0.2)
LOU:
yeah
(0.3)
LOU:
fine
(0.2)
LOU:
hhh ha ha .hhh=
INT:
=U:m
(1.0)
INT:
so I think (.) o:ne one of the things we can we can start with is if you th:ink of (.)
u:m like o:ne ↓friend of yours
(0.2)
INT:
l[ike a a] close friend
LOU:
[˚yeah okay˚]
(0.2)
INT:
u:m
(0.7)
INT:
a:nd
(0.4)
INT:
just tell me: a:h >how you met?<
(1.3)
LOU:
how we met=
INT:
=ye:ah
(.)
LOU:
well:
(0.2)
LOU:
we: live in ((HALLS OF RESIDENCE NAME)) ↓halls I don’t know
INT:
(nods)
(.)
LOU:
if you know where that is .hhh a:n:d
(0.2)
LOU:
she moved into our flat
(0.3)
LOU:
a week after everyone else because she didn’t like the people she was originally
living with
(.)
INT:
okay
(0.4)
LOU:
so ↑I first met her in our kitchen (.) unpakin
(0.7)
LOU:
then it were .hhh hhh
(0.2)
LOU:
it was quite strange because I though oh
(0.4)
LOU:
who’s she? ↑ahh cos we[’d only] been there a week so we’d already made like a
INT:
[˚ha ha˚]
little
(0.4)
INT:
rig[ht ˚okay˚]
LOU:
[bo:nd type] thing
(0.2)
INT:
how many [people] in the flat
LOU:
[˚n then˚]
(0.2)
LOU:
five
(0.2)
INT:
˚okay˚
(0.3)
LOU:
n so she was like the fifth person to move in
(0.7)
LOU:
n then we just started chattin ↓really and we found
(0.2)
LOU:
quite a bit to talk about and then it went from th↑e↓:re
(0.3)
LOU:
˚so like˚
(0.3)
LOU:
now we’ve formed like quite a good close friendship
(0.3)
LOU:
cos we both smoke you see so that was our little
(0.4)
INT:
he [he he he he ˚yeah˚]
LOU:
[bonding thing it] was like
(0.8)
LOU:
when we went out it was like d’you want to go outside for a fag n then
(0.5)
LOU:
that was [how] we started to bond r↓eally
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
(0.2)
LOU:
through that
(0.5)
LOU:
˚so˚ it can have its
(0.3)
LOU:
you know advantages smok↑in [ha ha .hhh] so ha ha [.hhh but] (.) yeah (.) now=
INT:
[ha ha ha xxx] [yeah absolutely]
LOU:
=we’re (.) quite close really I’m goin over to Ireland to see her in summer because
that’s where she lives ˚for a couple of weeks so˚=
INT:
=↑okay cool
(.)
LOU:
˚it’s [really good˚]
INT
[where about]s?
(0.2)
LOU:
e:m tsk
(0.7)
LOU:
not called is it ((LOCAL PLACE NAME))
(0.3)
LOU:
((LOCAL PLACE NAME))? (.) ((LOCAL PLACE NAME))?
(0.4)
LOU:
just outside Derry ˚I think it is˚=
INT:
=˚okay˚=
LOU:
=˚she lives˚
(0.4)
INT:
Northern [Ireland?]
LOU:
[˚so˚]
(.)
LOU:
yeah N[orthern Ireland yeah]
INT:
[˚xxx xxx xxx xxx] xxx˚
(0.9)
LOU:
˚s[o˚]
INT:
[o]:h nice one
(.)
LOU:
I kno(h)w=
INT:
=yea:h
(0.2)
LOU:
gets me a holiday anyway [doesn’t it ha ha] somewhere to stay hhh .hhh=
INT:
[ha ha ha ha ha]
INT:
=˚cool˚
(0.5)
INT:
so you’ve kno:wn her since s the start of this [year] xxx xxx xxx [Sep]temberish
LOU:
[˚xxx˚]
[˚xxx˚]
INT:
xxx
(0.2)
LOU:
feels like I’ve known her years though really=
INT:
=really?=
LOU:
=yeah
(0.4)
LOU:
like I know more about her than friends back home I think
(0.4)
LOU:
cos I think it’s cos you live together so=
INT:
=˚yeah˚=
LOU:
=you start to see sides of people you don’t (.) generally see
(0.4)
LOU:
˚in others˚
(0.7)
LOU:
cos you’re und under each others’ skin aren’t ya (.) so=
INT:
=˚yeah yeah˚ I know it [can be quite difficult]=
LOU:
[it’s crazy]
LOU:
=it is difficult but we’ve (.) we’ve learnt to live with each other (.) cos we know
when we need our own space n (.) we know when
(0.5)
LOU:
it’s time to be
(0.7)
LOU:
˚like with each other xxx xxx˚=
INT:
=.hhh when you are with each other what
(0.2)
INT:
I mean w what d’you get up to what d’you do?
(0.5)
LOU:
like in the day or::
(0.5)
INT:
we[ll]
LOU:
[js] generally
(.)
INT:
generally [in the day] in the evening or whatever (.) both
LOU:
[>well we generally<]
(0.2)
INT:
all thre[e]
LOU:
[x] typical watch telly togther an erm
(1.1)
LOU:
drink together before we go out ˚n then go out˚
(0.7)
LOU:
˚like˚
(0.4)
LOU:
do each other’s fake tan n stu HA ha ha ha .hhh so ha ha .hhh
(.)
INT:
d’you go out a l[ot?]
LOU:
[˚s˚]
(.)
LOU:
yeah
(0.2)
INT:
˚y[eah˚]
LOU:
[˚yeah˚] quite a lot
(0.3)
LOU:
but recently cos of exams we haven’t
(0.6)
LOU:
˚but then˚
(0.3)
LOU:
Wednesday night we had quite a ↓big night out
(0.6)
LOU:
so it was really good=
INT:
=that was the end of [exams]
LOU:
[yeah] that was (.) the last cos her last exam was on that day
as well so
(0.2)
INT:
˚okay˚=
LOU:
=it was good tim[ing ˚xxx˚]
INT:
[are you doing the same] course?
(0.2)
LOU:
no: she does business studies (.) so
(0.9)
LOU:
˚yeah˚
(0.2)
LOU:
˚she enjoys it˚
(.)
INT
˚okay˚
(0.8)
INT:
˚you ˚ ha [ha]
LOU:
[ha .hhh]=
INT:
=you sound unconv[inced? ha ha ha ha]
LOU:
[I know ha ha] .hhh it’s not my type of thing bu[t hhh]
INT:
[no:]
(1.2)
INT:
˚cool˚
(0.7)
INT:
and the the other people in the flat are you also close to th[em?]
LOU:
[y↑e]ah yeah we [are]
INT:
[˚xx˚]
LOU:
there’s one girl who
(0.9)
LOU:
isn’t she doesn’t come out with us cos she’s not a very: sociable person she’s the
type who’d rather stay in and watch films
(0.5)
LOU:
so we do watch films with her occasionally but=
INT:
=˚m[:˚]
LOU:
[she] goes home quite a lot you see
(0.5)
LOU:
so we don’t really see that much of her and she goes out with other friends from
other ha:lls so
(0.4)
LOU:
but the other four of us ↓we are
(1.2)
LOU:
quite [close]
INT:
[but th]is
(0.3)
INT:
but the girl from Ireland is more like
(0.3)
INT:
a part[icular friend yeah]
LOU:
[yeah parti]cular friend yeah
(0.2)
INT:
best
(0.3)
LOU:
be[st mate type] thing y[eah] yeah=
INT:
[right yeah]
[yeah]
LOU:
=she’s like if I had (.) there was something wrong with me or something she’s the one I’d go to out of the whole of the flat
(1.6)
LOU:
˚so (.) yeah she’s a particular one˚
(0.9)
INT:
and do you have friends outside the flat [as well]
LOU:
[y↑eah] y[eah]
INT:
[yea]h?=
LOU:
=there’s e:r we’ve fo:rmed like a group of about (.) fifteen of us
(0.5)
LOU:
we’re a:ll we’re all quite a close group cos we all live on the first floor you see
(0.3)
INT:
oh ri[:ght]
LOU:
[of] where we stay so
(0.6)
LOU:
there’s a couple who are from other ha:lls so it’s really good
(0.5)
LOU:
quite a sociable group n
(0.8)
LOU:
everyone (.) there’s people who are
(0.2)
LOU:
closer than others but then we’re all (.) close together
(0.6)
LOU:
˚it’s cool˚
(0.6)
LOU:
I like it=
INT:
=so are all are all that you:r (.) your little
(0.4)
INT:
˚I don’t know˚ cl↑an or what[ever]ha ha [ha ha]
LOU:
[yeah]
[poss]e ha h[a ha] ha .hhh [ha ha]
INT:
[posse]
[u:]m
(0.3)
INT:
are they all from outside Liverpool?
(0.4)
LOU:
yes (.) al[l of them]
INT:
[so] so so none of your friends are like
(0.7)
INT:
<locals> or none of your close friends are [li] like locals
LOU:
[no]
(0.3)
INT:
but you kn[ow] you know some people who are from Liverpool as well or?
LOU:
[˚x˚]
(.)
LOU:
e::m not really no there’s a (.) lad who’s from ((LOCAL PLACE NAME DELETED))
(0.7)
LOU:
which is
(0.2)
LOU:
nearest
(1.2)
LOU:
really but no one who actually l[ives in Liverpool]
INT:
[it’s not a lot further] th[an you i]s it?=
LOU:
[no]
LOU:
=n[(h)o it’s just like
ha .hhh]
INT
[xxx xxx xxx xxx xxx xxx] xx yeah=
LOU:
=yeah
(0.6)
INT:
˚yeah okay˚
(0.7)
LOU:
˚so˚
(0.4)
INT:
how how long d’you think you’ll
(0.7)
INT:
u:m
(0.7)
INT:
you’ll know these friends for d’you think
(.)
LOU:
hopefully
(0.6)
LOU:
like after university and stuff there’s a few that I think
(0.8)
LOU:
we’ll try and stay in touch with but we won’t (.) for obvious reasons but there’s a
few where
(0.7)
LOU:
I reckon we’ll stay
(0.3)
INT:
and what are [the ob]vious reasons?
LOU:
[cos we]
(0.3)
LOU:
like
(0.4)
LOU:
live
(0.6)
LOU:
too far away and ˚don’t˚
(0.4)
LOU:
˚know˚
(0.7)
INT:
˚m[: ˚]
LOU:
[˚x]xx˚
(0.5)
LOU:
like one (.) some of em are gonna move abroa:d n ˚stuff like that so˚
(0.7)
LOU:
˚I reckon we’ll try though˚
(0.2)
LOU:
it’s like [next] year we’re all getting houses
INT:
[˚x˚]
(0.4)
LOU:
but I think we’ll all stay in touch because
(0.2)
LOU:
that’s our group we’ve formed so we’re all living in different areas but
(0.4)
LOU:
I think we’ll stay in touch (.) I hope so anyway
(0.6)
INT:
have you got your
(.)
LOU:
yeah
(.)
INT:
house sorted out?
(.)
LOU:
yep
(0.3)
INT:
and you [>xx you gonna< be] staying with:
LOU:
[in ((LOCAL PLACE NAME))]
(0.3)
INT:
>some of the people you’re friends with now?<=
LOU:
=yeah=
INT:
=yeah?=
LOU:
=yeah=
INT:
=al[l from that sort of] g[roup?]
LOU:
[so it’s cool]
[yea]h
(0.4)
LOU:
we’re all in the same group apart from one girl which is a friend from back home
(1.0)
LOU:
erm [she’s] living with us as well
INT:
[˚okay˚]
(0.4)
LOU:
cos she hasn’t really ˚settled in properly˚
(0.3)
LOU:
˚with who she’s lived with˚ she’s not really fo:rmed ˚a friendship group so˚
(0.5)
INT:
>˚alright [okay˚<]
LOU:
[we’re like] takin her under our wing (.) n lookin after her ha
.hh[h]
INT:
[˚cool˚]
(0.2)
LOU:
˚so˚
(0.9)
LOU:
˚yeah˚
(0.2)
INT:
so tal talking bout friends from from before
(1.7)
INT:
u:m:
(0.9)
INT:
you came here straight after school right?
(0.5)
LOU:
from college y[eah]
INT:
[yea]h from college right
(0.3)
INT:
and you had a different
(0.6)
INT:
set of friends there obviously >how how< how in touch are you:: with th[em:]
LOU:
[very] LOU:
cos we:’ve got a very
(0.4)
LOU:
close group of friends there’s like six of us (.) including myself girls
(0.4)
LOU:
an (.) we’re the type of friends where we see each other every day
(0.5)
LOU:
we do stuff together every day so we’re still
(0.2)
LOU:
very close
(0.3)
LOU:
like I’ve been going home most weekends to spend time with them
(0.2)
INT:
↑alright [xxx]
LOU:
[n] like I ph↓one them in the week n
(0.5)
LOU:
it’s really strange cos I’ve got such a close group of friends back ho:me but I have
here that’s why: at first I didn’t (.) really want to come
(0.4)
LOU:
cos I was a bit sca:red of
(0.3)
LOU:
making new friends=
INT:
=˚yeah˚
(.)
LOU:
but it seems to have gone the opposite way
(0.4)
LOU:
˚x˚
(.)
INT:
˚xx˚
(0.6)
LOU:
cos we do depend on each other a lot back home
(0.6)
LOU:
like us six
(0.6)
INT:
˚hm:˚
(0.7)
LOU:
so
(0.2)
LOU:
it was [scary xxx xxx]
INT:
[and have your have] your::
(1.1)
INT:
friends from college met your uni friends
(0.7)
LOU:
e::m [>yeah<] they [come up o]n: Saint Paddy’s day for like the day out so they
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
[˚xxx˚]
LOU:
all
(0.6)
LOU:
like met n stuff they all got [on it was] quite stra:n:ge
INT:
[okay]
(0.7)
LOU:
it was like all my favourite people in one room: it was good=
INT:
=uh hu x[xx xxx xxx ye↑ah]
LOU:
[so xxx xxx xxx] ha ha .hhh it was dead goo:d
(0.4)
INT:
wha[t d’you do Pa]ddy’s day then
LOU:
[I liked it]
(0.6)
LOU:
˚what d’you think we done˚
(0.2)
INT:
ri[ght]
LOU:
[ha] ha [ha .hhh]
INT:
[where did you do it] h[a ha ha ha]
LOU:
[e::m] we start ↑do’you know where The Head of
Steam ↓is=
INT:
=hm m
(0.2)
LOU:
we started off there because obviously (.) that’s where our accommodation is
above there and then went into town ˚and then˚
(0.5)
LOU:
done like a bit of a bar crawl really
(0.6)
LOU:
n then went to a club afterwards
(0.3)
LOU:
˚and then˚
(1.3)
LOU:
spent the next day (.) in bed (.) ha [ha ha]
INT:
[ha ha ha] moaning=
LOU:
=yeah [˚he he˚ .hhh]
INT:
[ha ha ha ha ha] ha (.) cool
(0.5)
LOU:
[so] it was good
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
(0.3)
LOU:
˚it was really good˚ it was nice to see: (.) me fr different frien:d groups getting on
as well
(1.0)
LOU:
you know like so they could colli:de n
(1.0)
LOU:
˚xx˚=
INT:
=and are the:y (.) have you
(0.5)
INT:
have the two friends sort of: mixed >since< then as well or::
(.)
LOU:
e:::m wel[l they we’ve not really (.) been up n]o: they’ve not really had the
INT:
[xxx xxx had the opportunity]
LOU:
opportunity since that day
(0.7)
LOU:
but if (.) cos s:ometimes one cos they only live like an hour away one will pop up
for the night and then they’ll speak or sit in the kitchen n (.) ˚have like a cup of tea
n everything n˚=
INT:
=˚okay˚
(.)
LOU:
˚so it’s˚
(0.2)
LOU:
˚it’s really quite cool˚
(0.5)
LOU:
the girls are good who I live with like that they’re very: sociable as well so they
won’t just (.) ignor:e (.) someone [they’ll] come in ˚n˚
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.6)
LOU:
nice to meet yer ˚der der der so it’s co[ol˚]
INT:
[˚x]xx cool˚
(0.2)
LOU:
it’s really good
(1.7)
INT:
so wh↑at was it like
(0.5)
INT:
coming ↓here you said you were a bit sort of er:a:h[ah]
LOU:
[˚y]eah˚
(0.7)
INT:
u[:]m=
LOU:
[cos]
LOU:
=I wasn’t gonna: (.) I changed me mind about going to uni (.) over summer
(0.6)
LOU:
and then I got a summer job (.) and I really didn’t like it it [was not where I
INT:
[ha ha ha ha]
LOU:
wanted] to be at ↓all
(0.3)
LOU:
n I thought the only way I’m gonna get to where I wanna be is if I go to uni
(0.5)
LOU:
but I was (.) I was just scared of being l↑one↓ly
(0.3)
LOU:
I think ↓˚more than anything˚
(0.6)
LOU:
cos we are: really close back home
(0.5)
INT:
˚x[xx˚]
LOU:
[so i]t was one of them where:
(1.0)
LOU:
I just ↓thought I was gonna be on my own make no frien:ds
(1.5)
LOU:
it wa[s]
INT:
[w]ere you worried about losing contact [with xxx xxx xxx ˚college]
LOU:
[yeah definitely]
INT:
friends˚=
LOU:
=with cos
(0.4)
LOU:
two of them didn’t go to university (.) n three of them have done
(0.5)
LOU:
so it wa[s]
INT:
[˚r]ight˚
(1.5)
INT:
but that[’s not made a] difference reall[y?]
LOU:
[˚xxx xxx˚]
[n]o↑: not at a↑:↓ll
(0.3)
LOU:
like the t one of the girls (.) who stayed back ho:me at first she was a bit
(0.2)
LOU:
ooh you’re all makin new friends you’re gonna forget about us
(0.5)
LOU:
at the start she was a bit
(0.6)
LOU:
>but th[en<]
INT:
[˚m:]:[: ˚]
LOU:
[she’]s all right now
(1.4)
LOU:
she like=
INT:
=>but is she< making new friends
(0.3)
INT:
through wor:k n all that sort of stuff xx[x?]
LOU:
[er] ↑yeah yeah but she works with
(0.2)
LOU:
↓older people really
(0.2)
LOU:
[where she w]orks
INT:
[˚ri:ght˚]
(0.2)
LOU:
so:
(0.3)
LOU:
>she does<
(0.2)
LOU:
stay with us >but she’s got a< boyfriend as well so that sort of hel:ps so when
we’re not there she’s spending time w[ith hi]m
INT:
[˚x˚]
(1.0)
LOU:
so (.) she’s settled down a lot more now she just fe I think she felt a bit
(0.3)
LOU:
↓left out cos we were a:ll moving on with our lives and she felt like she was stuck
there:
(0.8)
LOU:
˚but˚
(0.3)
LOU:
˚she’s c[ome to te]rms with it now n˚ re(h)alised
INT:
[˚m hm˚]
(1.0)
LOU:
˚but˚
(0.6)
INT:
>that sounds like<
(1.9)
INT:
when you said you wanna sort of stay in touch with some of these university
frie[nd]s (.) you know
LOU:
[yeah]
(0.7)
INT:
for
(0.4)
INT:
ever or [or what] I: it sounds like sort of (.) the same with the ↑college
LOU:
[x yea:h]
INT:
friend[s xxx]
LOU:
[yea]:h definite[ly]
INT:
[these] are (.) like lifelong friends?=
LOU:
=definitely ˚yeah˚
(.)
INT:
and before coll↑ege like at school?
(0.4)
LOU:
it was (.) the sa↑:↓me
(0.3)
LOU:
f[riends xxx] college at school:l cos we all went to the [same college]
INT:
[cough cough]
[you all went to the same
INT:
school] and then the [same] college okay=
LOU:
[yeah]
LOU:
=so it was [really g]ood and there’s e:rm a couple of people from schoo:l (.) like
INT:
[all right]
(0.2)
LOU:
it’s a group of lads who I’ve kept in touch with as well
(0.3)
LOU:
cos they’re like ↓really close friends
(0.3)
LOU:
but it goes to show you when you move on who your friends ar:e
(0.7)
LOU:
cos
(0.3)
LOU:
they’re the people who wanna stay in touch and who make the effort to see yer
(0.5)
LOU:
and you make the effort to see them n it makes you rea[li]se (.) it’s like [xxx]
INT:
[˚xx˚]
[cos >I] INT:
mean there must have been other< people who you were
INT:
[friends with who have sort of fallen by] the way[side]
LOU:
[tsk yea:h (.) but they’ve just like]
[fizzle] yeah
(0.3)
INT:
why what sort of (.) what leads to that fizzling
(1.0)
LOU:
I think it’s mo:re a case of
(1.2)
LOU:
people (.) doin different thin:gs
(0.5)
LOU:
and then you don’t see each other often so you run out of
(0.3)
LOU:
li:ke a bit of conversation really and then
(0.3)
LOU:
>you just<
(0.3)
INT:
˚hm[:˚]
LOU:
[n]ever see them cos I’m he↓re
(0.5)
LOU:
hardly the people when I go home the the main friends who I (.) spend my time
with cos I’ve not got a lot of time (.) like for the weekend or something
(.)
INT:
˚hm:˚
(0.6)
LOU:
it’s rea[lly]
INT:
[>ca]n you think of< someone who’s >who’s like that that< maybe like a
school friend that you’ve
(1.4)
INT:
e[::r]
LOU:
[yea:]h
(1.0)
INT:
when >when when< did you last see them?
(0.8)
LOU:
I actually he↓:↑r
(0.4)
LOU:
cos we were really good friends at high school but we went to different colleges
(0.5)
LOU:
n
(0.5)
LOU:
she didn’t really associate with (.) the group of (.) you know the girls I’ve
m[et xxx] xxx xxx
INT:
[yeah]
(0.5)
LOU:
and I sa:w her I met up with her over Easter for a drink
(0.5)
INT:
˚ye[ah˚]
LOU: [it] was rea:lly good it was (.) but I hadn’t seen her for (.) since like schoo↑:↓l
(.) from then
(0.4)
INT:
oh r[ight okay]
LOU:
[so we went fro]m being like really really close to then nothing
(0.4)
LOU:
˚at ↑all:˚
(0.4)
INT:
had you both ch↑anged in that time though?
(0.3)
LOU:
e::m she’d dyed her hair
(0.3)
INT:
ha [ha ha ha ha]
LOU:
[but ha ha .hhh] ha ha .hhh (.) personality >but< I think she’d done a lot of
growin up
(0.3)
INT:
yea[h]
LOU:
[cos] when we were at school she was very
(0.5)
LOU:
like
(0.2)
LOU:
bad tempered
(1.0)
LOU:
like could (clicks fingers) you know (.) but
(0.4)
LOU:
th- s:tubborn as well but she (.) she admitted that as well she said I was
(0.2)
LOU:
terrible at school
(0.5)
LOU:
moo:d swings n stuff so she’s d[one a lot of] growin up
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
(0.7)
LOU:
even though she’s only five foot nothing sti(h)ll [ha ha ].hhh she’s still dead
INT:
[ha ha]
LOU:
small
(0.7)
INT:
but it w↑asn:’t awkward seeing her (.) cos I thought oh xx
(0.5)
LOU:
cos we had loa:ds to talk about >cos we hadn’t seen each other for two years but<
(0.2)
LOU:
other people you see
(0.6)
LOU:
and it’s like >they’re not< interested so you’ve got
(0.2)
LOU:
˚nothing to talk about˚
(0.9)
LOU:
˚s[o˚]
INT:
[th]ey’re not interested in?
(.)
LOU:
like what you’ve been doing they’re just
(0.6)
LOU:
like HI n then walk away
(0.4)
INT:
˚xxx˚=
LOU:
=>they’re not< they don’t stop for a conversation n stuff
(0.7)
LOU:
but then in the same respect I could stop them n
(0.6)
LOU:
˚it’s˚
(0.4)
LOU:
quite strange isn’t it? (.) yeah
(.)
INT:
yeah
(1.1)
LOU:
˚it’s really quite strange˚
(1.5)
INT:
yeah I know I recognise these things [you’re saying] (.) yeah
LOU:
[yeah]
(1.1)
LOU:
˚so like˚
(0.5)
LOU:
where did that little bond go?
(0.3)
LOU:
˚it’s wei[rd˚]
INT:
[˚y]eah˚
(0.7)
INT:
↑are you friends with people from also (.) um:
(0.6)
INT:
from class (.) and er from
(0.5)
INT:
the course as as [well] >other than< >other than< your your=
LOU:
[yeah]
LOU:
=it’s
(0.5)
LOU:
i[t’s quite strange]
INT:
[fifteen strong p]oss[e]
LOU:
[y]eah (.) cos it’s li(h) ha ha .hhh it’s my own fault really
because I’ve got the:m
(1.0)
LOU:
I don’t really make as much effort as I should have done with class mates which is
one of the things I’m gonna try and do next year
(0.4)
LOU:
but there’s
(0.2)
LOU:
two girls who are (.) I see quite a lot
(0.4)
LOU:
like we don’t just
(0.5)
LOU:
˚go˚ like we went out for each other’s bir:thday n: [stuff like that n]
INT:
[˚alright okay] yeah˚
(0.5)
LOU:
phone each other up n
(0.3)
INT:
˚x˚
(0.6)
LOU:
arh exams n hhh
(0.4)
LOU:
but that’s ↑only like two of them and there’s another gi:rl who lives in my halls who does n (.) I saw her last night actually
(0.3)
LOU:
so there is a few people that I speak to but I never
(1.6)
LOU:
like (.) text em n: say you coming out tonight apart from like them two
(0.2)
LOU:
˚who I’ve˚
(0.7)
INT:
˚yeah˚=
LOU:
=˚made friends with˚ (.) but one of em
(0.2)
LOU:
went to my coll↑ege
(1.1)
LOU:
so it was [xxx like yea:h so I kn]ew who she wa:s [but]
INT:
[alright so you sort of knew them]
[>but] you weren’t< ↓friends
INT:
with her at [college you just just sort of knew to see]
LOU:
[no: we’re just (.) like acquaintance type] thing [˚yeah˚]
INT:
[˚xxx xxx˚]
(1.1)
LOU:
˚so˚
(1.1)
INT:
and were they people >xxx like< research methods cl↑ass or some[thing like]
LOU:
[yeah]
tha:[t?]
LOU:
[y]eah]
(0.4)
INT:
so that’s the [one where you actu]ally see mo[st of them I guess]
LOU:
[xxx xxx xxx]
[yeah where you see like in]
LOU:
lectures it’s difficult cos you just walk in sit down n walk back out
(0.3)
INT:
˚hm m[:˚]
LOU:
[>s]o it’s<
(0.2)
LOU:
difficult >you can’t really< s have a conversation with people ˚can yer˚?=
INT:
=˚yeah˚
(0.3)
INT:
˚no˚
(.)
LOU:
but in the tutorials it’s a lot easier
(0.9)
LOU:
˚x[x]x˚
INT:
[˚x˚]
(0.4)
LOU:
˚make it˚ that’s what I’m gonna try and do next year make more of an effort ˚with
people˚
(0.8)
INT:
˚okay˚=
LOU:
=˚in our course˚ but then
(0.9)
LOU:
it’s wei:rd because then I spend most of me time
(0.4.)
LOU:
with people
(1.0)
LOU:
in ha:lls
(0.9)
LOU:
˚so˚
(0.8)
LOU:
plus I’ve already=
INT:
=˚hm m˚
(.)
LOU:
made a bond with them n
(0.2)
LOU:
they’re my friends now (.) so
(1.2)
LOU:
˚be quite˚
(0.4)
LOU:
˚st[range˚]
INT:
[you’re talk]ing about them like they’re sort of two different groups?
(0.4)
LOU:
yea[h]
INT:
[of] friends as >it it< feels like tha[t? xxx yeah?]
LOU:
[yeah definite]ly
(0.6)
LOU:
like
(0.2)
LOU:
people on my course are a different group to people ˚who I live with˚
(0.6)
LOU:
˚definite[ly˚]
INT:
[˚hm] m˚
(0.8)
LOU:
I can act differently with them as well
(0.5)
LOU:
like [I act di]fferently with people on my course than I do
INT:
[yeah?]
(1.0)
LOU:
˚with˚
(0.5)
INT:
like how so
(0.7)
LOU:
like I’m more::
(0.4)
LOU:
more confident with people
(0.5)
LOU:
I live with
(0.3)
LOU:
you know to be yourself like sometimes you hold back don’t you n
(.)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(.)
LOU:
things I’m like that with people on my course >I I< have to get to know them
before I let them get to know me
(0.6)
LOU:
it’s quite strange
(0.3)
LOU:
don’t know why I do it but eh heh .hhh hhh
(0.2)
LOU:
it’s like if I go in to you know when you start a new job?
(0.5)
INT:
yeah
(.)
LOU:
tsk and everyone’s already for:med
(0.4)
LOU:
their friends that’s when I’m more:
(0.6)
LOU:
like self conscious n
(0.3)
INT:
˚hm [m˚]
LOU:
[a] bit
(0.9)
LOU:
cos you’re going into a new group whereas at uni it’s quite easy because
(0.2)
LOU:
everyone’s in the same position
(0.5)
LOU:
so
(0.9)
LOU:
you just
(0.2)
LOU:
be yourself that’s all you can be
(0.2)
INT:
yeah
(1.1)
INT:
do you get that feeling
(0.4)
INT:
that
(1.0)
INT:
people already knew each other on the course more than in halls where everyone
was strangers?
(0.3)
INT:
or was it the same sort of thing=
LOU:
=it was the same sort of thin:g it’s just
(0.7)
LOU:
I see
(0.2)
LOU:
them people more: so it give me [more] time to
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.6)
LOU:
n plus we were out socialising so
(0.2)
LOU:
and when you drink[ you get a] lot more confidence
INT:
[˚hm m˚]
(0.3)
LOU:
to just
(1.1)
LOU:
chat really .hh[h] hhh
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.3)
LOU:
˚so˚
(0.2)
INT:
>I mean<
(0.2)
INT:
yes you do >I mean has has that< bee:n (.) sort of quite an important part of
(0.3)
LOU:
yeah=
INT:
=meeting people and all that?=
LOU:
=the socialising’s side [cos the first] (.) day we moved in the first thing we done
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
LOU:
was
(0.3)
LOU:
opened a bottle of wi:ne n
(1.8)
LOU:
chatted n then went out >cos you’re just<
(0.3)
LOU:
jus more loose with yourself aren’t yer?[ ˚ yer like˚]
INT:
[yea]h (.) yeah
(.)
LOU:
not afraid to say stuff
(0.2)
INT:
˚x˚ (.) hard to do that in le[ctures]
LOU:
[yea]:h definitely [ha ha .hhh]
INT:
[ha ha ha ha] ha
(0.6)
LOU:
takes a good couple of hours where in lectures you haven’t got that time have
[you it’s like two minute]s ha ha .hh[h]
INT:
[˚no xxx xxx xxx˚]
[˚x]x[x˚]
LOU:
[s]o
(0.7)
INT:
˚xx˚
(.)
LOU:
can’t really drink either can yer ha ha
(0.2)
INT:
we↓:ll
(0.3)
INT:
I hope not h[a ha ha ha ha ha]
LOU:
[ha ha ha ha ha ha]
(0.5)
LOU:
.hhh
(1.2)
LOU:
˚x˚=
INT:
=um
(2.3)
INT:
>you you talk about<
(0.9)
INT:
like sort of making pla:↓ns for n:ext year >n I [mean you] see you’ve< (.) brought
LOU:
[yeah]
INT:
up next year ˚w˚ like twice once is moving into
(.)
LOU:
yeah
(.)
INT:
moving into a house (.) n like finding
(0.3)
INT:
house mates n all that sort of stuff
(0.7)
INT:
um n that sounds pretty s that’s all pretty sorted?
(.)
LOU:
yeah
(.)
INT:
>how many xx how many are< they g gonna be in the house?
(.)
LOU:
there’s
(0.4)
LOU:
two there’s two houses of three where we’re next door to each other (.)
s[o three of us are] in one n three of us are in the other
INT:
[all right okay]
(0.4)
LOU:
girls
(0.2)
LOU:
cos we thought six girls in one house and one bathroom
(1.0)
LOU:
no wa(h)y ha ha .hhh just wouldn’t work would it really (.) s[o]
INT:
[n]o it doesn’t=
LOU:
s[o we] thought if we live next to each other we’re still together but
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
(0.3)
INT:
two bathroom[s]
LOU:
[ye]ah exactl[y]
INT:
[˚y]es˚
(0.8)
INT:
˚h[m˚]
LOU:
[s]o
(0.3)
INT:
one of my first digs was
(0.2)
INT:
two guys and eight girls with one bathroom
(0.8)
LOU:
nightmare
(0.3)
INT:
big bathroom but doesn’t matter [ha ha ha ha ha]
LOU:
[yeah ha ha ha] .hhh (.) so that was all right
(0.2)
LOU:
but I’m thinking next year maybe (.) I want to live in a bigger house because (.)
now’s the only opportunity
(0.2)
LOU:
you’re gonna get to live with
(0.5)
LOU:
that amount of people
(0.2)
INT:
˚hm˚
(0.6)
LOU:
so I think next year I’m not gonna
(0.3)
LOU:
th:ink about the bad things >˚I’m gonna think about the good things< n˚
(1.0)
LOU:
cos this year it was like .hhh not enough bathroom spa:ce
(0.2)
(the following utterance is a kind of ‘extension’ noise; an ‘and so on, and on’ type utterance)
LOU:
xxxxxx
(0.7)
LOU:
˚whereas next year I’m just gonna be˚
(0.9)
LOU:
˚go for it˚
(0.6)
LOU:
but then the work’s
(0.6)
LOU:
the good thing about (.) that is (.) cos there’s only three of us we’re gonna do our
wor:k
(0.7)
INT:
yeah
(0.3)
LOU:
we’re not gonna be distracted or:
(0.7)
INT:
how’s that been this year?
(1.0)
LOU:
e:↑:m not too bad really cos when I’ve got work to do I go to the library
(0.6)
LOU:
so that I shut myself away n (.) spend all day there
(0.3)
INT:
˚h[m˚]
LOU:
[˚n˚]
(0.4)
LOU:
everythin but >it’s just at< ni:ght
(0.6)
LOU:
like you’re tempted to go out when really should get up ˚in the morning n do˚ it’s
just
(1.4)
LOU:
n then we go round n watch TV together n
(0.5)
LOU:
you can [end up] being there all night ˚n˚
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.5)
LOU:
˚when you’ve got things to do but˚ you kn you know when you’ve gotta do something so you know when
(0.5)
LOU:
like
(0.2)
LOU:
enough’s enough
(1.2)
LOU:
˚really˚
(0.3)
LOU:
well I do anyw[ay so] hhh ha ha .hhh=
INT:
[yeah]
INT:
=you looked a little glum saying that [ha ha ha ha]
LOU:
[>I know< ha ha ha]
(0.4)
LOU:
.h[hh]
INT:
[much] rather be going out [(.) ha ha] ha
((interviewer uses ‘comedy’ voice))
LOU:
[but hhh]
(0.5)
LOU:
you know when you’ve gotta do something don’t yer (.) when [something’s] gotta be done
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
(0.8)
INT:
hhh[ xx]
LOU:
[it’s like] with revision I stayed in for two weeks n
(1.0)
LOU:
like just pf focused on that
(0.3)
LOU:
didn’t really see much ˚of anyone˚
(0.6)
LOU:
˚n it’s benefited really˚ (.) well I hope so ˚find out˚
(0.4)
INT:
[find out soon enough yeah]
LOU:
[xxx xxx ha ha .hh]h soon find [out]
INT:
[xxx] xxx were your other friends of the same sort of
(0.2)
LOU:
yea[h]
INT:
[m]indset ˚x x x˚ where [everyone xxx]
LOU:
[especially] in our flat it was like right
(0.2)
LOU:
>like wh if one of us woke up earlier than the others we’d< wake each other up n
(0.9)
LOU:
have break[fast] n then we’d
INT:
[˚okay˚]
(0.2)
LOU:
˚revise˚
(0.4)
INT:
so it’s actually quite supportive=
LOU:
=yea[h it was really]
INT:
[rather than being a] distraction
(.)
LOU:
yeah it was really goo[d]
INT:
[˚ye]ah˚
(0.5)
LOU:
everyone was like that ˚really˚
(0.6)
LOU:
and we’d have little breaks together: n
(1.8)
LOU:
˚like every couple of hours n stuff˚ so it was really good
(0.2)
INT:
˚xx˚=
LOU:
=that’s why I didn’t go to the library really >cos I thought< if I go the library I’d just be sat there thinking .hhh
(0.2)
LOU:
I wonder what they’re doing ha ha ha ha .hhh bu[t it was really good n then]
INT:
[xxx xxx know they’re just
INT:
sitting] at their own[ de]sk doing their o(h)wn [thing and that’s not that
LOU:
[˚yeah˚]
[yeah exactly]
INT:
inte]resting after all
(0.3)
INT:
h[a ha ha]
LOU:
[askin each] other questions n stuff so it was real[ly go]od
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.2)
LOU:
it was quite helpful
(0.6)
INT:
but you’re not n ne th there’s no one
in your: flat that’s on the same course or
anyth[ing]
LOU:
[n]:[o]
INT:
[so] you weren’t like sharing >˚wo[rk with them or] anything okay˚<
LOU:
[no]
(1.0)
INT:
.hhh ↑um (.) talking about like f:riendship ↓more generally?
(0.4)
LOU:
yea↑h
(0.8)
INT:
˚w˚
(0.4)
INT:
<what>
(0.2)
INT:
<does it mean> to you to be friends with someone?
(0.3)
LOU:
everything I think (.) really
(0.9)
LOU:
li:ke
(2.0)
LOU:
e:r
(0.4)
LOU:
it’s jus f- s-
(0.5)
LOU:
just
(0.2)
LOU:
I’d <rather>
(0.9)
LOU:
sit
(0.2)
LOU:
with someone cos I h↑ate to be on my ow↓:n
(0.5)
LOU:
I’m one of those people who I like to be round people and interacting
[with p]eople all the time so it does mean quite a lot to me
INT:
[yeah]
(0.6)
LOU:
plus yer I think ↓you need friends it’s like a necessity in life
(0.7)
LOU:
like you nee::d
(0.8)
LOU:
jus f[or xxx]
INT:
[what do you need friend]s for?
(0.4)
LOU:
like
(0.8)
LOU:
suppo↑:rt
(1.)
LOU:
˚n˚
(0.5)
LOU:
someone to rely ↑on n trust and just someone to have
(0.6)
LOU:
a good t↑ime with n spend your life with really
(.)
LOU:
isn’t it?
(0.9)
LOU:
˚like˚ (.) I’d rather
(0.2)
LOU:
spend my life with some
(0.8)
LOU:
someone than on my own
(0.4)
INT:
˚m:˚
(0.6)
LOU:
so it’s quite impo[rtant to me]
INT:
[.hhh have there] been have there been times this year when
you’ve needed
(0.3)
INT:
support from your friends
(1.1)
LOU:
e:↑::m
(0.6)
LOU:
I think it was when I was coming to uni ↓really
(0.6)
LOU:
that was the main (.) cos I needed to know (.) that they’re still gonna be (.) like my
friends back home are [still gonna] be there n
INT:
[˚okay yeah˚]
(1.3)
LOU:
˚definitely˚
(.)
INT:
˚ye[ah˚]
LOU:
[but ]nothing serious has really happen:ed
(0.2)
LOU:
YET anyw(h)ay touch wo[od where I nee]:d
INT:
[touch wood hhh]
(0.4)
LOU:
˚really need them˚
(0.4)
LOU:
but I just need em for ↓everyday ↑things
(0.6)
LOU:
like they just (.) >mean quite a lot to me< my friends do
(0.8)
LOU:
they like me family really
(0.7)
LOU:
like an extended family (.) s(h)o
(0.7)
LOU:
˚to me˚
(1.1)
LOU:
so I find it quite sad when I see people who
(0.5)
LOU:
are quite lonely n stuff a-
(0.5)
LOU:
cos for me that would be the worst situation to be in
(0.9)
LOU:
like
(1.3)
LOU:
don’t know about yourself but
(0.6)
LOU:
it’s very
(0.5)
INT:
yeah
(2.5)
INT:
yeah I mean there’s always a couple (.) n [I guess] I guess you see them you
LOU:
[yeah]
INT:
see them n[ xxx course] xxx n all that sort of stuff they don’t [seem to] engage
LOU:
[like in yeah]
[they jus]
INT:
[with peo]ple ˚xxx˚ stuff
LOU:
[yeah]
(0.7)
LOU:
it’s quite sad really I just
(0.4)
LOU:
like our flat mate I do try n make the effort with her cos I just think
(0.3)
INT:
this is the [the quieter one yeah]
LOU:
[must be yeah must] be so hard for her
(0.6)
LOU:
cos we’re all quite (.) bubbly
(0.3)
LOU:
characters n
(0.6)
LOU:
˚x˚
(0.4)
INT:
n is she unhappy?
(0.6)
LOU:
she doesn’t seem to ↓be
(0.2)
LOU:
cos I’ve spoken to her about
(0.3)
LOU:
th↓ings l↑i:ke
(0.5)
LOU:
how are you finding it here n stuff n she
(0.2)
LOU:
quite l↑ikes it
(0.3)
INT:
˚m[:˚]
LOU:
[she]’s on- thinking about changing her course next year
(0.4)
LOU:
she’s not too
(0.2)
LOU:
settled with the course but she seems okay
(0.6)
LOU:
n she’s living with friends next ye↓ar
(0.3)
LOU:
cos I was worried she’d be on her own again in our [ha:ll]s
INT:
[yeah]
(0.3)
LOU:
˚so I spoke to her about that˚
(1.4)
INT:
˚good okay˚
(0.9)
INT:
UM
(3.3)
INT:
can you th::ink o↓:f:
(1.4)
INT:
like (.) a particular
(0.3)
INT:
um act of kindness
(0.7)
INT:
that that a friend <has>
(0.9)
INT:
has e:r (.) has done for you or >sort of [something a<] friend has done for you or
LOU:
[xxx xxx]
INT:
something where the friend’s been particularly
(0.7)
INT:
kind to you
(0.5)
LOU:
em
(4.1)
LOU:
don’t don’t r↓eally kn↑ow
(2.2)
LOU:
just trying to think of like an example
(0.5)
LOU:
˚in my head˚
(5.7)
LOU:
just like times when you’re upset
(0.8)
LOU:
n they’re there for you
(0.7)
LOU:
n
(0.7)
LOU:
jus general things like that really cos they’re all quite
(0.7)
LOU:
kind friends ↓mine are=
INT:
=yeah
(.)
LOU:
very (.) supportive n
(1.0)
LOU:
if there’s something wrong: they’ll
(0.5)
LOU:
help you out
(0.7)
LOU:
but
(0.2)
LOU:
g[en]erally (.)˚like˚
INT:
[˚x˚]
(0.2)
LOU:
we all do stuff for each other
(0.5)
LOU:
˚like kind things˚
(0.6)
LOU:
like my friend’s uncle
(0.6)
LOU:
passed away last week n it was his funeral on Tuesday
(0.6)
LOU:
and >obviously couldn’t be there cos at university we’ve had exams n stuff so it
was just like little phone< ca:lls
(0.3)
LOU:
to like
(0.2)
LOU:
show your support n stuff
(2.5)
LOU:
li[ke you] get the same back as well
INT:
[what]
(0.6)
LOU:
so [it’s not] just li↓:ke I’m giving giving giving your getting
INT:
[yeah]
(1.0)
LOU:
˚you’re getting support back as well˚
(0.4)
INT:
and you find that with all your friends it’[s sort of equally give and tak]e
LOU:
[yeah definitely yeah]
(1.1)
INT:
what what does it what does it ↓<mean support?>
(1.0)
INT:
like t t >sort of<
(0.4)
INT:
to
(1.2)
INT:
support a friend what what’s: >I dunno know< what’s involved with that
(0.2)
LOU:
it’s it can just be a conversation
(0.4)
LOU:
or even just li:ke >a little bit of< affection
(1.1)
LOU:
n the look they give you that they know
(0.7)
LOU:
something just saying like it’s gonna be all right
(0.4)
LOU:
˚it’s like˚ just affection really
(0.2)
INT:
˚hm˚
(1.4)
LOU:
cos we are quite close we do like sit n cuddle n stuff
(0.2)
LOU:
like
(0.4)
LOU:
which you don’t ↓normally see but
(0.4)
LOU:
us girls we’re like (.) quite close in that
(0.7)
LOU:
respect so it’s easy to see: if something’s up with someone
(0.2)
INT:
yeah=
LOU:
=cos you know them that well
(0.9)
LOU:
so it is just like affection n
(0.9)
LOU:
just little like expressions they give yer
(0.6)
LOU:
˚so you know˚
(1.7)
INT:
especially the ones you live with xx[x xxx xxx you] get to know them
LOU:
[yeah exactly yeah]
(0.7)
((2 MINUTES 49 SECONDS DELETED))
(4.4)
INT:
have you ever been let down by a friend?
(0.4)
LOU:
yeah
(1.7)
INT:
that was quite defin↑ite
(.)
LOU:
yea(h)h hhh [hhh]
INT:
[hhh] hhh hhh hhh hhh hhh
(0.5)
LOU:
yeah
(0.2)
LOU:
I have
(0.2)
INT:
can you tell me about that?
(0.2)
LOU:
e::m
(0.8)
LOU:
well she was
(0.6)
LOU:
we were very: >you know when you have one of those< friends who (.) we’ve
been friends since nursery
(0.4)
INT:
˚x[x˚]
LOU:
[so] like we have
(0.3)
LOU:
actually gro[wn up togther]
INT:
[you’ve grown up together] quite [literally yeah]
LOU:
[xxx yeah] literally
(.)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(0.4)
LOU:
an:
(0.6)
LOU:
it was through a boyfriend she ˚betrayed me˚
(0.2)
LOU:
˚she sort of like˚
(0.4)
LOU:
˚them two˚
(0.9)
LOU:
˚sort of (.) had intercourse together n˚
(0.6)
LOU:
˚behind me back˚
(0.7)
LOU:
so it was quite a
(1.1)
LOU:
dumph on m[y:]
INT:
[what] a boyfriend of <your[s>] that she
LOU:
[yeah]
(.)
LOU:
she did
(0.2)
INT:
˚pilfered˚
(0.2)
LOU:
yeah ha [ha .h]hh
INT:
[ha ha ha]
(0.2)
LOU:
s[o]
INT:
[ho]w long ago was that?
(0.3)
LOU:
e:m
(1.1)
LOU:
˚s bout˚ (.) two ↑years ago now
(0.3)
INT:
˚oka[y˚]
LOU:
[t]wo or three years ago
(0.8)
INT:
so is
(0.6)
INT:
college
(0.2)
INT:
type
(0.2)
INT:
era
(0.3)
LOU:
er yeah just before the year just before college [it was so] GCSE time
INT:
[okay (.) alright]
(0.2)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(0.6)
LOU:
n that did affect me (.) looking back now it really did affect me
(.)
INT:
ho[w wh what s]ort of
LOU:
[quite badly]
(0.3)
LOU:
like psychologically like NOW
(2.8)
LOU:
I find it ha:rd
(0.2)
LOU:
harder to trust people
(0.6)
LOU:
in that aspect
(0.2)
INT:
˚hm˚
(1.0)
LOU:
n her: it’s like
(0.3)
LOU:
cos it wasn’t the fact it was him it was who
(0.3)
LOU:
done it like it was that particular ↓frien:d
(0.3)
LOU:
the one who I’d grown up with who I knew th- who knew me best
(0.7)
INT:
y[eah]
LOU:
[it was just] a proper
(1.2)
LOU:
li:ke
(1.0)
LOU:
we relied on each other for e- like people used to say oh where’s your partner in
crime (.) if they saw me to me and the same to her like they expect the other one
to be round the corner
(.)
INT:
˚hm˚=
LOU:
=it was really
(0.7)
LOU:
it was really difficult
(1.0)
LOU:
˚it’s diff (.) difficult time˚
(.)
INT:
are you in touch with her now?
(.)
LOU:
ye↑ah wh sh actually living with us next ye(h)ar s(h)o .hhh it’s quite stra:nge it’s
(0.2)
LOU:
she’s the girl who’s not settled down properly here
(0.7)
INT:
oh [you you] mentione[d her ear:l]ier yeah ˚yeah˚
LOU:
[so]
[yeah (.) yeah]
(.)
LOU:
it’s her she e:m
(1.0)
LOU:
but then other fr- if I talk about it like the girl I was on about before Anna
(0.2)
LOU:
who
(0.2)
LOU:
we were talking about who lives in Ireland=
INT:
=˚hm˚
(.)
LOU:
she <th:inks>
(0.2)
LOU:
like I’m ↑too s↑oft (.) and I’m too
(1.0)
LOU:
>like she said if it was< he:r she (.) wouldn’t look at her again
(0.7)
LOU:
but then I think that’s my problem I’m too soft n too
(0.9)
LOU:
like I can’t just forget about someone who was that close to me
(0.9)
LOU:
˚you know for doing something like that˚ (.) I’d rather
(1.0)
LOU:
put it behi:nd
(0.6)
INT:
you don’t think it’s too soft
(0.6)
LOU:
but
(0.3)
LOU:
.hhh
(0.6)
LOU:
no:
(0.6)
LOU:
but then when I talk to people about I s:it there n think ˚maybe I am being ↑too
soft˚
(1.4)
LOU:
˚d’you see what I mean˚
(.)
INT:
.hhh I [do see what you] mean and I’m trying to
LOU:
[it’s like]
(0.2)
INT:
I wanna make you say it though ha [ha ha ha ha ha]
LOU:
[xxx ha ha] .hhh
(0.2)
LOU:
it’s
(0.2)
LOU:
it’s quite stra:nge
(0.9)
LOU:
but
(1.2)
LOU:
>I don’t know I just didn’t< want something like that to come between us
(1.7)
LOU:
but then it is really quite a
(0.7)
INT:
I [take it that]
LOU:
[I know it doesn’t s]ound
(.)
INT:
she n: (.) the ex-boyfriend are not s still together [˚or anything˚]
LOU:
[no they w]ere but that was the LOU:
annoying thin:g she come to me when he split up with her
(1.7)
LOU:
s[o I was li:]ke
INT:
[<really>]
(1.0)
LOU:
that’s when we di[d fall out (.) n she’s l]ike
INT:
[that’s quite cheeky ha ha ha]
(0.3)
LOU:
hu
(0.8)
LOU:
what do you want me to say?
(0.9)
LOU:
I just s:aid to her like I hope he was worth it
(0.9)
LOU:
˚like˚
(0.6)
LOU:
cos she kno:w:s that er
(0.5)
LOU:
like
(0.5)
LOU:
I’ve forgiven her but I’ll never forget it but in a s↑ense[ I don]’t think that’s really
INT:
[˚hm˚]
LOU:
forgiveness
(1.1)
LOU:
˚in a way˚
(0.2)
LOU:
cos there is
(1.0)
LOU:
like NOW
(1.7)
LOU:
I won’t (.) there’s things I won’t
(0.7)
LOU:
tell her
(0.2)
LOU:
˚n˚
(0.6)
LOU:
it’s affected >little things like that so it has< aff[ected ]our friendship (.) quite=
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
LOU:
=badly but
(0.6)
LOU:
then she’s only got herself to blame really I can’t be blamed for that
(0.7)
INT:
has it affected the way
(0.7)
INT:
th:at um >you speak to other< friends o:r
(0.6)
INT:
or or or the way you’ve made other fr↑iendships or anything like that?
(0.5)
LOU:
e:m
(0.5)
LOU:
not in sense of that way but (.) sometimes I do
(2.0)
LOU:
it affects ↑boyfriends really
(0.6)
LOU:
having trust in them
(0.3)
LOU:
rather than
(0.4)
LOU:
friends
(0.6)
LOU:
which is quite strange really
(.)
INT:
even [though it was y- y- y- y-] yah (.) [yeah]
LOU:
[cos it’s like I blame her mo:re] [but I’m li]ke
(1.0)
LOU:
takin it out on them rather than
(0.6)
LOU:
my friends
(0.6)
LOU:
but it’s because I think the group of friends I’ve got now
(0.3)
LOU:
I
(0.2)
LOU:
do trust them
(0.7)
LOU:
but then I trusted he:r so (.) it’s one of them things that er
(0.7)
LOU:
you’ve just gotta
(1.2)
LOU:
not think it
(1.6)
INT:
[what (.)] consciously not [think] it?
LOU:
[˚xxx˚]
[yeah]
(1.0)
INT:
˚s˚ do you find it sort of bubbling up n then you [go n n] n no [I’m not going
LOU:
[yeah]
[yeah (.) yeah]
INT:
down the]re (.) ah
(.)
LOU:
definitely
(0.8)
INT:
˚okay˚
(0.6)
LOU:
˚definitely˚
(3.1)
LOU:
˚but yeah˚ I think
(0.4)
LOU:
that’s the only time my friend’s proper let me down
(0.6)
LOU:
but it was quite a
(0.9)
LOU:
˚like˚ doosh
(0.6)
LOU:
but [er I thin]k if I wasn’t as strong minded as I was (.) back then
INT:
[˚xxx˚]
(0.3)
LOU:
I wouldn’t
(0.5)
LOU:
it would have affected me quite
(0.8)
LOU:
like severely
(0.4)
LOU:
I know it might seem to you like a little thin:g (.) but to me it [was]
INT:
[oh g]od
INT:
no [n n n n no] Jesus no
LOU:
[no ha .hhh]
(0.2)
LOU:
˚x˚ it was quite like
(0.5)
LOU:
˚a big thing˚
(1.0)
LOU:
˚but˚=
INT:
=˚no absolutely˚
(0.2)
INT:
.hhh I mean I guess it’s it’s why
(2.3)
INT:
I’m curious about
(0.5)
INT:
her moving in: with [you next year .hhh >is she gonna be] in the same< house?
LOU:
[yea:h that’s what she said]
INT:
or[: next] door one the s[ame one]
LOU:
[yeah]
[same h]ouse
(0.4)
LOU:
cos that’s what Anna was saying like how can you
(0.3)
LOU:
be so supportive of her cos she’s not really made
(0.4)
LOU:
she only stays at uni when she can n then she goes back h↓ome
(0.5)
LOU:
sh[e’s] like I don’t see how you show her that much suppor:t n
INT:
[˚m˚]
(0.4)
LOU:
look after her in a way when she’s done ↑that to yer
(0.7)
INT:
d’[you see a] lot of her?
LOU:
[˚xxx˚]
(0.9)
LOU:
I ↑used to but n:ot so much now
(0.7)
LOU:
like I used to before the all this happened we used to be on the phone every day
(0.3)
INT:
yeah
(0.2)
LOU:
we used to
(0.2)
LOU:
go like socialise together every night as you do when you’re younger
(0.6)
LOU:
but we went different high schools but she was still my closest friend
(1.2)
LOU:
˚xxx˚
(1.0)
LOU:
so then through college I hardly saw her really
(0.3)
LOU:
jus
(0.9)
LOU:
just sort of let her
(0.3)
LOU:
drift apart n then we [come ↓he]↓re
INT:
[˚hm m˚]
(1.0)
LOU:
˚n˚
(0.5)
LOU:
˚just had to (.) sho[w her a bit of support really˚]
INT:
[how much have you seen xxx how] much have you seen of her
here?
(0.6)
LOU:
tsk e:m well
(0.4)
LOU:
she does I do ((DEGREE SUBJECT A)) with ((DEGREE SUBJECT B))
(.)
INT:
yeah
(0.5)
LOU:
n she does ((DEGREE SUBJECT B)) s↓o we see each other on our c↑ourse
(0.3)
INT:
oka[y]
LOU:
[but] then
(0.3)
LOU:
outside that I don’t
(1.0)
INT:
xx[x xxx]
LOU:
[really] see her
(0.5)
INT:
she’s one of that sort of course work
(0.2)
LOU:
y[eah]
INT:
[that cour]se gr[oup of friends rather th]an
LOU:
[yeah now she is]
(0.3)
LOU:
but it’s quite stra:nge from how close we used to be to now [she’s ju]st
INT:
[yeah]
(1.4)
LOU:
˚like˚
(1.6)
LOU:
but then I when e:m
(0.7)
LOU:
it was not so long ago me n Anna were talking about it actually
(0.6)
LOU:
n she was saying how can you live with her next year n stuff cos I don’t really
(0.8)
LOU:
like bringing it up in a way
(0.4)
LOU:
cos it’s just
(1.0)
LOU:
something
(0.7)
LOU:
that happened
(0.9)
LOU:
you know what I mean
(0.6)
LOU:
so [I’d only]
INT:
[you don’t] not like to bring it up
(0.6)
INT:
because it’s still uncomfortable for [you? or be]cause of how people
LOU:
[yeah]
(0.9)
INT:
like what Anna says you’re a bit soft n [all that sort of] stuff=
LOU:
[yeah]
LOU:
=I think [it’s]
INT:
[>other] people’s reactions< or or your: <own> feelings [about it]
LOU:
[bit of bo]th
re[ally]
INT:
[bit of] both
(0.3)
LOU:
cos when they say that it makes me thin:k how I should have reacted
(0.8)
LOU:
n how
(1.7)
LOU:
like
(0.3)
INT:
how should you have reacted
(0.8)
LOU:
˚jus˚
(1.1)
LOU:
not been friends wi(h)th her a(h)ny more really .hhh I was qu- I were r- really
was quite soft about it
(0.9)
LOU:
˚but˚
(1.4)
LOU:
˚she’s my fr↓iend˚
(0.4)
LOU:
so in some respects I do think
(0.8)
LOU:
I
(0.3)
LOU:
well with he:r I give a lot more than she gives back to me
(1.8)
LOU:
but then
(1.3)
LOU:
like she knows it now
(1.0)
INT:
yeah=
LOU:
=like she know:s
(1.1)
INT:
xx [>I mean it] must have< changed the way that she:
LOU:
[xxx]
(1.2)
INT:
deals with ↑you as well I [guess]
LOU:
[yea]h
(1.3)
LOU:
˚definitely˚
(0.2)
INT:
how’s how’s that?
(0.6)
LOU:
˚it˚
(1.6)
LOU:
it’s quite str- at f↑i:rst it didn’t like
(0.2)
LOU:
especially when she come to me after they:’d >spli- I don’t think<
(0.4)
LOU:
<I think she doesn’t realise what she’s done>
(1.6)
INT:
↑really?
(.)
LOU:
yeah I think that’s what it is I [think] ↓she just thinks of it as li:ke
INT:
[˚xx˚]
(1.1)
LOU:
something that’s in the past and it is forgotten about whereas it really
(0.2)
LOU:
isn’t
(1.4)
INT:
d’you t↑alk about it with her
(1.2)
LOU:
˚no˚
(1.7)
INT:
it just doesn’t come ↑up o[r:]
LOU:
[yea]h it just doesn’t=
INT:
=˚okay˚
(1.0)
LOU:
it’s li:ke a taboo subject ˚in a way˚
(0.7)
INT:
a[lright okay]
LOU:
[just never] comes up
(0.2)
LOU:
˚so˚
(0.6)
LOU:
a[fter it happened a couple of months after]
INT:
[an and if it did you’d sort o]f:
(0.4)
LOU:
yeah
(.)
INT:
change the s[xx xx topic]
LOU:
[we did have] a: (.) like quite a bit of an argument about it
(0.2)
LOU:
a couple of months after it happen:ed
(0.4)
LOU:
but then since then ˚we’ve never mentioned it˚
(1.7)
LOU:
˚jus˚
(0.2)
LOU:
˚one of [those things˚]
INT:
[sounds a little] uncomfortable I [guess]
LOU:
[yea:]hr [it is] ha ha .hhh=
INT:
[yeah]
INT:
=˚yeah˚
(0.5)
LOU:
˚it’s quite strange˚
(0.9)
LOU:
˚but˚
(2.1)
LOU:
ma[ybe] it will come up when we’re living together
INT:
[um]
(0.7)
LOU:
˚who knows˚
(0.6)
INT:
I guess it’s b↓ound to over [the course of] the year n all that [sort of stuff]
LOU:
[yeah]
[definite]ly
(1.8)
LOU:
˚definitely˚
(1.7)
LOU:
um
(0.6)
INT:
˚sort of˚
(3.6)
INT:
I think I’ve finished with most of the questions but there’s s- c-
(0.2)
INT:
couple of things
(1.0)
INT:
((cough cough)) sorry
(0.5)
INT:
u[m]
LOU:
[>it’s alright<]
(0.9)
INT:
tsk
(1.7)
INT:
can you sum up what friendship
(0.5)
INT:
means to you?
(1.5)
LOU:
˚like in:˚
(1.3)
LOU:
the only word I can think of really is everything
(0.5)
INT:
yeah?=
LOU:
=like
(0.3)
LOU:
everything
(2.2)
LOU:
cos (.) you’ve got your family n you’ve got your friends n
(1.4)
INT:
>do your fam- do your family< know: a lot of your friend[s n ]stuff yeah?=
LOU:
[yeah]
LOU:
=yeah
(0.2)
INT:
[↓so there’s quite a] quite a lot of overlap=
LOU:
[definitely]
LOU:
=yeah
(.)
INT:
do they know your uni friends as well as your college friends
(.)
LOU:
e::m they’ve m↑et (.) like my m↓um’s met them when she’s come up n
(0.8)
LOU:
you know for the day to see me or:
(0.3)
INT:
okay
(0.3)
LOU:
like if she’s (.) bringing me foo:d or if she’s taking stuff back
(1.0)
LOU:
bu[t they’ve not] come back to m↓ine (.) with me (.) or any[thing yet s]o
INT:
[˚xxx xxx˚]
[>alright okay<]
(0.3)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(0.6)
LOU:
c[os it]
INT:
[but th]e college friends I guess they would have
(0.3)
LOU:
yeah=
INT:
=you would have se[en a l]ot more
LOU:
[yeah]
(0.2)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(1.0)
LOU:
˚so [xxx˚]
INT:
[are you gonna] see o over the summers n that are you gonna see:
(0.4)
INT:
and you’re
(0.3)
INT:
spending some time with
(0.2)
INT:
Anna[: ˚n all that sort of stuff˚]
LOU:
[well there’s actually]
(0.2)
LOU:
↓the group’s going to Ireland
(0.2)
LOU:
we’re sta[ying in erm]
INT:
[oh right okay the] whole bunch
(0.3)
LOU:
yeah this is (.) before I’m going to Anna’s us lot are going but we’re going the
south of Ireland
(0.4)
LOU:
we’re staying in like ↑a holiday home
(0.7)
LOU:
like a cottage [so]
INT:
[okay]
(0.8)
LOU:
we’re all doing that together that’s in Ju:ne
(1.1)
LOU:
so I don’t know whether (.) we’ll do anything (.) ↑hopefully (.) like a couple of
us’ll meet up or something
(.)
INT:
˚xxx˚
(0.6)
LOU:
but (.) quite a few people live down south n then there’s
(.)
LOU:
me who lives up north so
(0.6)
LOU:
ha ha
(.)
INT:
are any of the others also from the north?
(0.5)
LOU:
e:m ↑yeah there’s (.) a couple of ↓people so I’ll probably see more like there’s a
girl I’ve
(0.4)
LOU:
become really good friends with he:re and she lives round the corner from me at
home
(0.3)
INT:
alright [okay]
LOU:
[like li]terally round the corner so=
INT:
=hhh
(.)
LOU:
I’ll probably see a [lot of her]
INT:
[you’ve only me]t her here at at [in the halls]
LOU:
[I saw I] knew who she was at ↓college
(0.4)
LOU:
but you know when[ you]
INT:
[oh th]is is the xxx xxx the the [xxx xxx different girl okay]
LOU:
[no that’s a different one (.) LOU:
yea]h
(.)
INT:
alright
(0.5)
LOU:
n then
(1.1)
LOU:
I sort of introduced her to one of our lad friends n now they’re together s[o]
INT:
[˚m]:˚
(.)
LOU:
see quite a lot of her n
(0.4)
LOU:
formed quite a good friendship
(0.2)
INT:
.hhh you’ve spoken
(0.2)
INT:
um:
(0.6)
INT:
>I’m glad you mentioned that actually because you’ve< spoken about friends
(0.9)
INT:
almost exclusively girlfriends
(0.2)
LOU:
yeah hhh=
INT:
=you’ve not really spoken about friendships with with boys at [↑all]
LOU:
[yeah]
(0.3)
INT:
d’you have ↑d’you have male fr[iends?]
LOU:
[yeah] definitely
(0.4)
LOU:
there’s a: (.) actually there’s a lad who: in halls who a- (.) is like
(0.3)
LOU:
my brother from another mother as they say
(.)
INT:
hhh=
LOU:
=we are very
(0.4)
LOU:
cos I do (.) I find it (.) I know I’ve just talked about girlfriends [but] they are like
INT:
[yeah]
(1.0)
LOU:
I find it easier to talk to lads than I do to ↑girls
(0.8)
INT:
re[ally]
LOU:
[d’y] yeah=
INT:
=okay
(0.2)
LOU:
but I think it’s because I know how judging girls can be
(0.7)
LOU:
because before I come to uni I was very like
(0.8)
LOU:
I judge a book by its cover
(0.5)
LOU:
whereas now I’ve learnt to open my mind a bit n
(1.0)
LOU:
start actually talking to different people n I’ve found
(0.6)
LOU:
like quite a
(0.4)
LOU:
good friends n different [peop]le
INT:
[yeah]
(1.0)
LOU:
and have like different (.) dress sense n
(0.6)
LOU:
music type
(0.7)
LOU:
˚found good friends in them so˚
(0.4)
INT:
not all [sort of cookie] cutter type
LOU:
[xxx xxx x]
(.)
LOU:
n yeah
(.)
INT:
yeah
(0.2)
LOU:
w[e’re a]ll
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.3)
LOU:
like people in our group we’re so different to each other but we all connect in our
own way
(0.4)
INT:
like what are [some] of the extremes
LOU:
[˚xx˚]
(.)
LOU:
like
(0.6)
LOU:
there’s a gi:rl who’s very (.) hippy
(0.8)
LOU:
very very hippy n she does creative writing so she’s very
(0.8)
LOU:
dada:::
everything’s a flower n ha [ha ha .hhh] n
INT:
[ha ha ha ha ha]
(0.6)
LOU:
then there’s people who are quite
(0.9)
LOU:
like
(0.2)
LOU:
Jimmy his name is who’s quite serious n (.) quite
(0.3)
LOU:
speaks proper n
(0.6)
LOU:
n they get on ↑it’s really good
(0.4)
LOU:
to see how a variety of people
(0.4)
LOU:
like if you saw a picture of us it would be (.) like whoa
(0.6)
LOU:
cos
(0.4)
LOU:
just different varieties o[f per]sonality there
INT:
[˚x˚]
(0.9)
LOU:
but it’s really cool cos we all have our little connection so
(1.1)
LOU:
˚it’s dead good˚
(0.7)
INT:
.hhh [so]
LOU:
[˚x]xx˚
(0.2)
INT:
male friends
(0.2)
LOU:
m
(0.4)
INT:
how have you: how have you met
(0.4)
INT:
male friends
(1.3)
INT:
through ha:lls? or[::]
LOU:
[y]eah (.) through ↓here
(.)
INT:
y[eah]
LOU:
[through] halls ↑yeah
(0.5)
LOU:
well the first person I spoke to was one of the male friends cos I was waiting
downstairs for my mum and dad to park the car like blah di blah
(0.5)
LOU:
n then we just started speaking n then
(0.5)
LOU:
we went to the student union the night after
(0.5)
LOU:
n I saw them n then that’s how our group formed cos
(0.4)
LOU:
they knew so and so and so and so knew so and so
(0.5)
INT:
˚ok[ay˚]
LOU:
[but] there’s (.) ↓here
(0.8)
LOU:
in our I think there’s about
(1.9)
LOU:
↑eight or nine lads n then
(0.4)
LOU:
there’s only like six of us girls so it is (.) more ↓male
(0.3)
LOU:
dominated our group
(0.3)
INT:
alright o[kay]
LOU:
[s]o
(.)
INT:
˚okay˚=
LOU:
=n I do fi:n:d I do find it easier to talk to
(0.7)
LOU:
˚men than women really˚
(1.4)
LOU:
˚so˚
(0.4)
LOU:
like [Phillip the lad]
INT:
[so it’s the six of you] girls that are taking on these houses next [year]
LOU:
[yea]h
(1.2)
INT:
n the blokes are they gonna be
(0.7)
LOU:
th[ey’re: four] of them are living together
INT:
[around xx]
(0.9)
LOU:
n another three are living together
(0.4)
LOU:
n then a couple of the others are just (.) you know with friends off their ↑cour:se
(0.6)
LOU:
so
(.)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(1.4)
LOU:
m
(0.5)
INT:
˚okay˚
(0.2)
LOU:
˚it’s quite strange˚
(1.4)
INT:
the >how how< do the
(0.4)
INT:
<friendships>
(0.9)
INT:
differ between
(0.5)
INT:
the guys and the girls
(1.4)
LOU:
<e[r]m>
INT:
[˚for you˚]
(2.1)
LOU:
it
(0.3)
LOU:
it ↑doesn’t really it’s
(2.5)
LOU:
I don’t know
(0.9)
LOU:
like with Phillip we’re:
(0.5)
LOU:
very close like
(0.8)
LOU:
he if Anna wasn’t round I’d go to him rather than the girls in me flat if there was
something wrong with me=
INT:
=˚okay˚
(0.6)
LOU:
he’d be
(1.3)
LOU:
or if I’m annoyed at something I’d go round and speak to him about it or:
(1.2)
LOU:
cos he is
(1.8)
LOU:
without Anna (.) he is ˚like my best mate at uni that I’ve made really˚
(0.6)
LOU:
but then ↑all of them
(0.9)
LOU:
like I could spen:d
(0.4)
LOU:
time with every individual one of them
(0.2)
INT:
yea[h]
LOU:
[in] the same way
(2.0)
INT:
˚xx˚=
LOU:
=so I am but (.) think Anna n
(0.2)
LOU:
Phillip are the ones I’m more: close to
(1.4)
LOU:
it’s like (.) yesterday: I was ill
(0.6)
LOU:
>cos we ce went out celebratin so I didn’t<
(0.5)
LOU:
[<stay in my flat>]
INT:
[when you say ill you mean] you mean seriously hung o[ver]
LOU:
[ye]ah [ha ha
INT:
[ha ha ha ha ha ha
LOU:
<serious]l(h)y>=
INT:
ha ha ]
INT:
=let’s not beat around the [bush here ha ha ha]
LOU:
[n ha ha ha] .hhh n I went round his and we watched a
film n just like
(0.6)
LOU:
chilled out
(0.3)
LOU:
whereas I didn’t
(0.2)
LOU:
stay in my flat (.)˚with my girls˚
(0.4)
INT:
˚m:˚=
LOU:
=n we do a lot of cooking together
(0.2)
LOU:
like we cook for each other (.) mo[re than] we do ˚for the girls in the flat˚
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.7)
LOU:
he is like ˚a really close f˚ >like when you< said for a friend to think of
(0.5)
INT:
[it was it was him or her yeah]
LOU:
[I didn’t know which one (.) yeah] it was
(0.5)
LOU:
which one
(0.7)
INT:
˚oka[y˚]
LOU:
[>bu]t then: when you said how did you meet them I couldn’t remember how I met him so it was like Anna<
(.)
LOU:
[xxx ha ha ha]
INT:
[ha ha ha ha ha] ha ha ha=
LOU:
=.hhh
(0.2)
LOU:
we were talking about that the other day actually (.) >we were like< how did we meet?
(1.2)
LOU:
˚it’s [quite˚]
INT:
[>you] don’t kno[w?<]
LOU:
[c]an’t can’t actually remember how we started to
(0.9)
LOU:
the only:
(0.2)
LOU:
the
(0.5)
LOU:
like
(0.4)
LOU:
first thing I could think of was going to the pub to watch the footba:ll and he was
there
(0.9)
LOU:
but cos he lives next door: I suppose
(0.2)
LOU:
we just sort of
(1.2)
LOU:
x[xx xxx xxx xxx]
INT:
[Phillip’s got the flat] next door: [in in] hal[ls]
LOU:
[yeah] [in] halls [so]
INT:
[˚ok]ay˚]
(1.0)
LOU:
it’s like I’ll wake up n if none of me flat’s up I’ll go round his for like
(0.5)
LOU:
↑cup of tea: (.) n
(0.9)
LOU:
so it’s really quite good
(0.4)
LOU:
I think I’ll probably see him over summer
(0.6)
LOU:
cos he doesn’t live too far from me so
(0.3)
INT:
˚alright [okay˚]
LOU:
[probably gonna] see him for ˚the day or something˚
(1.1)
LOU:
˚like˚
(0.3)
LOU:
˚go out there˚
(1.6)
LOU:
˚xx˚
(0.5)
INT:
.hhh >is there anything< about (.) friends or friendship that
(1.7)
INT:
u:m
(0.5)
INT:
>that you can think of that< I’ve not asked about? or or that I’ve just sort of
s:kipped over or that you’ve g[ot that] you particularly want to say?
LOU:
[e:m]
(2.8)
LOU:
↑no not really I don’t think so
(1.2)
LOU:
just think
(2.0)
LOU:
<they are a necessity in life>
(0.5)
LOU:
˚friends˚
(0.6)
LOU:
˚definitely˚
(0.4)
INT:
what would you do without them
(3.8)
LOU:
probably be best friends with my mum ha [ha ha] .hhh b(h)ut .hhh
INT:
[˚ha ha˚]
(0.3)
LOU:
I don’t k↑now I I wouldn’t know what to do without them
(.)
INT:
˚x˚
(0.7)
LOU:
cos I do most things with them
(0.5)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(1.2)
LOU:
really (.) I couldn’t imagine not
(0.4)
LOU:
being with my friends
(0.2)
INT:
you said you don’t spend much time alone
(0.2)
LOU:
yea:h I don’t=
INT:
=>d’you spend any time< alone
(0.5)
INT:
apart from [studying]
LOU:
[I was] thinking about it the other day really and I don’t
(0.8)
LOU:
it’s quite stra:nge
(1.2)
LOU:
it’s like cos if I’m not with my friends I’m with my boyfriend or I’m with my
mum and dad
(0.9)
LOU:
˚so˚ I do get the odd (.) but I think that’s one thing I need to do really (.) spend
more time
(0.4)
LOU:
>on my [own< but] then I just
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.5)
LOU:
don’t see the point
(0.8)
LOU:
plus I think too much as well so
(0.3)
INT:
hhh
(0.3)
LOU:
ha ha=
INT:
=can’t have that [not at uni]vers[ity ha ha ha] ha ha=
LOU:
[yeah] [I know ha ha]
LOU:
=think about things too much so=
INT:
=how does the boyfrien::d frien:ds (.) dynamic work out for you=
LOU:
=well:
(0.6)
LOU:
he:
(0.7)
LOU:
I met him through a (.) a f- close friend’s brother
(0.4)
LOU:
one of the girls back home
(0.4)
INT:
˚okay˚=
LOU:
=so
(0.5)
LOU:
n then her brother also
(0.2)
LOU:
started (.) seeing another frien:d in our group so we all are
(1.2)
LOU:
like one big group
(0.4)
LOU:
there’s like ↑nine of us I think cos there’s another lad as well so it’s like us six
girls n them three lads and we are the same group
(0.4)
LOU:
like he’ll go out with my friends when I’m not there cos they’re his friends too=
INT:
=yeah
(0.4)
LOU:
so it’s really cool [n then]
INT:
[>is he here in] Liverpool<
(0.3)
LOU:
no: em he’s from back home
(0.4)
INT:
righ[t]
LOU:
[s]o n then when he come up here he <formed>
(0.2)
LOU:
like
(0.2)
LOU:
a close
(0.2)
LOU:
group with the lads he
(0.2)
LOU:
got on with them dead well so
(0.5)
LOU:
it was really ˚quite comforting˚
(0.3)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(.)
LOU:
˚to see that˚
(1.5)
LOU:
˚so˚ it’s not difficult cos one of my friends her boyfrien:d
(0.7)
LOU:
he doesn’t come out with us and she finds it quite difficult because she has to (.)
divide her ti:me
(0.3)
INT:
m[::]
LOU:
[whereas I] don’t if
(0.5)
LOU:
my friends wanna go ou:t he’ll come with us or
(1.0)
LOU:
cos like Michael’s there as well so we are just like one big group reall[y]
INT:
[y]eah=
LOU:
=˚n he’s included in it˚
(1.6)
LOU:
˚so it’s quite good˚
(0.5)
INT:
but you still get to spend time with the boyfriend=
LOU:
=yeah definitely [we] still do stuff to our
INT:
[yeah]
(0.3)
LOU:
our[selv]es n
INT:
[˚yeah˚]
(0.3)
INT:
˚yeah˚
(0.7)
LOU:
˚definitely˚
(0.5)
INT:
˚okay˚
(.)
LOU:
n then he’ll come up he:re n stay over
(0.5)
LOU:
n we’ll just stay in and do our own thing or
(1.5)
INT:
˚um˚
(0.2)
LOU:
˚so it’s good˚
(1.8)
INT:
I don’t th:ink I’ve got anything else to a:sk
(2.3)
LOU:
I feel like I’ve gone on forever ha ha
(.)
INT:
well you’re supposed to s[o xxx xxx ha ha ha ha ha]
LOU:
[xxx that’s alright then ha ha ha] .hhh
(0.2)
INT:
<erm:>
(0.6)
INT:
.hh[h okay >if you don’t have anything] else< to to ↓add shall we shall we wrap it
LOU:
[feels strange not asking]
INT:
up there?
(.)
LOU:
↑yeah yeah that’s
(0.3)
LOU:
yeah=
INT:
=cool
(0.5)
INT:
>th[ank you very mu]ch<=
LOU:
[xxx xxx]
LOU:
=okay you’re welcome=
INT:
=xxx xxx xxx
(0.6)
LOU:
[xxx xxx drink hhh]
INT:
[xxx xxx xxx y]ea(h)h
(1.2)
INT:
that’s still: (.) ˚recording xxx xxx˚
(2.9)
INT:
make sure I find my xxx otherwise
*END*
PAGE
1
Answers
0No answers posted
Post your Answer - free or at a fee
Login to your account tutor account to post an answer
Posting a free answer earns you +20 points.
LoginTo get help with a similar task to What Is The Meaning Of Friendhship: Conduct An Inductive Thematic Analysis On The Data. Lab Report., Ask for Psychology Assignment Help online.